Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Orthographic learning in dyslexic Spanish children

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Reading fluency is one of the basic processes of learning to read. Children begin to develop fluency when they are able to form orthographic representations of words, which provide direct, smooth, and fast reading. Dyslexic children of transparent orthographic systems are mainly characterized by poor reading fluency (Cuetos & Suárez-Coalla 2009; Spinelli, De Luca, Di Filippo, Mancini, Martelli, & Zoccolotti, 2005; Wimmer, 1993). Therefore, the main problem for these children could be the difficulty in developing orthographic representations of the words they read. The aim of this study was to test the ability of dyslexic Spanish-speaking children (whose native language is Spanish) to develop orthographic representations and determine if the context helps them. For this, two experiments were conducted with a group of 100 children, 7–12 years of age. The groups were comprised of 20 dyslexics, 40 chronological age-matched controls and 40 reading level-matched controls. In the first experiment, eight unfamiliar words (four short and four long) were presented six times within the context of a story. In the second experiment, eight pseudowords were presented on a computer and the children had to read them aloud. In both experiments, the reading and articulation times of experimental and control stimuli were compared, before and after the training. Children without dyslexia showed a decrease of the influence of length of word on reading speed, indicating a lexical reading, while for dyslexic children, the influence of length remained unchanged. These results appeared when the stimuli were presented in the context of a story as well as when presented in isolation. In short, our results describe that dyslexic children of transparent orthographic systems have problems in developing orthographic representations of words.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Archer, N., & Bryant, P. (2001). Investigating the role of context in learning to read: A direct test of Goodman’s model. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 579–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J., & Muller, D. (2005). Phonological recoding and rapid orthographic learning in third-graders’ silent reading: A ciritical test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 203–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics’ phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874–888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cao, F., Bitan, T., Chou, T., Burman, D. D., & Booth, J. R. (2006). Deficient orthographic and phonological representations in children with dyslexia revealed by brain activation patterns. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(10), 1041–1050.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castejón, L., González-Pumariega, S., & Cuetos, F. (2011). Reading fluency acquisition in a sample of Spanish children: A longitudinal study. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 34(1), 19–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements-Stephens, A. M., Materek, A. D., Eason, S. H., Scarborough, H. S., Pugh, K. R., Rimrodt, S., et al. (2012). Neural circuitry associated with two different approaches to novel word learning. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 99–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuetos, F., & Suárez-Coalla, P. (2009). From grapheme to word in learning to read in Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 583–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuetos, F., Rodríguez, B., Ruano, E., & Arribas, D. (2007). Prolec-R, Batería de evaluación de los procesos lectores, Revisada. Madrid: TEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: a quick measure of reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(4), 733–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E. (2006). Accounting for children’s orthographic learning while reading text: Do children self-teach? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95, 56–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., Perry, K. E., Stanovich, K. E., & Share, D. L. (2002). Orthographic learning during reading: Examining the role of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82(3), 185–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, P. F., & Share, D. L. (2007). Orthographic learning during oral and silent reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 55–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L., & Saltmarsh, J. (1995). Beginning readers outperform older disabled readers in learning to read words by sight. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 295–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C., & Roberts, K. T. (1979). Do beginners learn printed words better in contexts or in isolation? Child Development, 50, 675–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1980). The influence of orthography on readers’ conceptualization of the phonemic structure of words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 371–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(1), 116–124(9).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, J. R., & Kroll, J. F. (1976). Spelling and sound: Approaches to the internal lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 361–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogaboam, T. W., & Perfetti, C. A. (1978). Reading skill and the role of verbal experience in decoding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 717–729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kyte, C. S., & Johnson, C. J. (2005). The role of phonological recoding in orthographic learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 166–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landi, N., Perfetti, C. A., Bolger, D. J., Dunlap, S., & Foorman, B. R. (2006). The role of discourse context in developing word form representations: A paradoxical relation between reading and learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 114–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maloney, E., Risko, E. F., O’Malley, S., & Besner, D. (2009). Tracking the transition from sublexical to lexical processing: On the creation of orthographic and phonological lexical representations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(2), 858–867.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R. (1985). Acquisition of word identification skills in normal and disabled readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 78–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martens, V. E. G., & de Jong, P. F. (2008). Effects of repeated reading on the length effect in word and pseudoword reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(1), 40–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Angell, P., & Castles, A. (2007). Orthographic learning via self-teaching in children learning to read English. Effects of exposure, durability and context. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 71–84.

  • Orsolini, M., Fanari, R., Tosi, V., De Nigris, B., & Carrieri, R. (2006). From phonological recoding to lexical reading: A longitudinal study on reading development in Italian. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(5), 576–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oullette, G., & Fraser, J. R. (2009). What exactly is a yait anyway: The role of semantics in orthographic learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 239–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A. (2007). CheckVocal: A program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from DMDX. Behavior Research Methods, 39(4), 859–862.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramos, J. L., & Cuadrado, I. (2006). PECO (Prueba para la evaluación del conocimiento fonológico. [Test for assessing the phonological awareness]). Madrid: Instituto de Orientación Psicológica EOS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma, P. (1983). Printed word learning in beginning readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 321–339.

  • Reitsma, P. (1989). Orthographic memory and learning to read. In P. G. Aaron & R. M. Joshi (Eds.), Reading and writing disorders in different orthographic systems (pp. 31–44). New York: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricketts, J., Bishop, D. V. M., Pimperton, H., & Nation, K. (2011). The role of self-teaching in learning orthographic and semantic aspects of new words. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(1), 47–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Santamaría, P., Arribas, D., Pereña, J., & Seisdedos, N. (2005). Evaluación Factorial de las aptitudes Intelectuales (Factorial assessment of intellectual skills). Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Shalev, C. (2004). Self-teaching in normal and disabled readers. Reading and Writing, 17, 769–800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72(2), 95–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2004). Orthographic learning at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(4), 267–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2008). Orthographic Learning, phonological recoding, and self-teaching. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior (Vol. 36, pp. 31–82). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinelli, D., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Mancini, M., Martelli, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2005). Length effect in word naming in reading: Role of reading experience and reading deficit in Italian readers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(2), 217–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2012). Reading strategies in Spanish developmental dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 62(2), 71–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Flectcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 2–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, H., Castles, A., & Nickels, L. (2012). Rapid communication. Word regularity affects orthographic learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(5), 856–864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, H., Castles, A., Nickels, L., & Nation, K. (2011). Context effects on orthographic learning of regular and irregular words. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(1), 39–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2001). Escala de Inteligencia para Niños-Revisión. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S. (1997). Differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 50A, 439–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by Grant PSI2012-31913, 2013–2015, from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paz Suárez-Coalla.

Appendix 1

Appendix 1

Table 4 Stimuli used in the experiments

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Suárez-Coalla, P., Ramos, S., Álvarez-Cañizo, M. et al. Orthographic learning in dyslexic Spanish children. Ann. of Dyslexia 64, 166–181 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-014-0092-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-014-0092-5

Keywords

Navigation