Skip to main content
Log in

The effects of phonological transparency on reading derived words

  • Part IV Relating Oral Language Abilities To Reading
  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to determine whether poor readers have more pronounced problems than average-reading peers reading derived words the base forms of which undergo a phonological shift when a suffix is added (i.e., shift relations as in “natural”), as compared to derived words whose forms are phonologically and orthographically transparent (i.e., stable relations, as in “cultural”). Two computer-based word recognition tasks (Naming and Lexical Decision) were administered to children with reading disability (RD), peers with average reading ability, and adults. Across tasks, there was an effect for transparency (i.e., better performance on stable than shift words) for both child groups and the adults. For the children, a significant interaction was found between group and word type. Specifically, on the naming task, there was an advantage for the stable words, and this was most noteworthy for the children with RD. On the lexical decision task, trade-offs of speed and accuracy were evident for the child reader groups. Performances on the nonwords showed the poor readers to be comparable to the average readers in distinguishing legal and illegal nonwords; further analyses suggested that poor readers carried out deeper processing of derived words than their average reading peers. Additional study is needed to explore the relation of orthographic and phonological processing on poor readers’ memory for and processing of derived words.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Balota, D. A. (1990). The role of meaning in word recognition. In D. A. Balota, G. B. Flores d’Arcais, & K. Rayner (eds.), Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 9–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (1994). Multiple orthographic and phonological codes in literacy acquisition: An evolving research program. In V. W. Berninger (ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 277–319). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D. (1999). Basic processes in reading: Mulitple routines in localist and connectionist models. In R. M. Klein, & P. McMullen (eds.), Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia (pp. 413–458). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12(3–4), 169–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F., & Nomanbhoy, D. (1993). Phonological and morphological development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 177–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F., & Stone, C. A. (in press). The effects of morphological structure on children’s reading of derived words. In E. Assink & D. Santa (eds.), Reading complex words: Cross-language studies. Kluwer.

  • Carreker, S. (1999). Teaching reading: Accurate decoding and fluency. In J. R. Birsh (ed.), Multisensory teaching of basic language skills (pp. 141–182). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B., Davies, P., & Richman, B. (1971). Word frequency book. NY: American Heritage Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Champion, A. H. (1997). Knowledge of suffixed words in reading and oral language contexts: A comparison of reading disabled and normal readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 47, 29–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, P., Beauvillain, C., & Segui, J. (1989). On the representation and processing of prefixed and suffixed derived words. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L., & Carlisle, J. F. (1994). Speed of word recognition as a distinguishing characteristic of reading disabilities. Educational Psychology Review, 6, 115–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudell, R. G. (1976). Naming of objects by dyslexic and other learning-disabled children. Brain and Language, 3, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derwing, B. B. L., Smith, M. L., & Wiebe, G. E. (1995). On the role of spelling in morpheme recognition: Experimental studies with children and adults. In L. B. Feldman (ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 3–27). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test, 3rd edition. NY: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1997). Sight word learning in normal readers and dyslexics. In B. Blachman, (ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia (pp. 163–189). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., & Arnbak, E. (1996). The role of morpheme recognition and morphological awareness in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 209–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, A. E., & Liberman, I. Y. (1995). The role of phonology and orthography in morphological awareness. In L. B. Feldman (ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 157–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, C., Napps, S., & Feldman, L. B. (1985). Relations among regular and irregular morphologically related words in the lexicon as revealed by repetition priming. Memory and Cognition, 13, 241–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P. (1989). Levels of affixation in the acquisition of English morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 519–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, M. K. (1988). Beyond phonics: Integrated decoding and spelling instruction based on word origin and structure. Annals of Dyslexia, 38, 259–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K. (1989). Productive knowledge of derivational rules in poor readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 39, 94–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, A. M. (1989). The alphabetic principle and learning to read. In D. Shankweiler & I. Y. Liberman (eds.), Phonology and reading disability (pp. 1–34). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M., Lacerenza, L., & Borden, S. L. (2000). Putting struggling readers on the PHAST track: A program to integrate phonological and strategy-based remedial reading instruction and maximize outcomes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 458–476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R. (1985). Acquisition of word identification skills in normal and disabled readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(1), 78–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moats, L. (2000). Speech to print: Language essentials for teachers. Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., Anderson, R., Schommer, M., Scott, J. A., & Stallman, A. C. (1989). Morphological families and word recognition. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 262–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition., In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royer, J. M., & Sinatra, G. M. (1994). A cognitive-theoretical approach to reading diagnostics. Educational Psychology Review, 6, 81–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., Katz, L., Fowler, A. E., Liberman, A. M., Brady, S. A., Thornton, R., Lundquist, E., Dreyer, L., Fletcher, J. M., Stuebing, K. K., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1995). Cognitive profiles in reading disabled children: Comparison of language skills in phonology, morphology, and syntax. Psychological Science, 6(3), 149–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., Share, D., & Geva, E. (1995). Evidence for superior orthographic skills in dyslexics. Psychological Science, 6(4), 250–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singson, M., Mahoney, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and morphological skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 219–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoltz, J. A., & Feldman, L. B. (1995). The role of orthographic and semantic transparency of the base morpheme in morphological processing. In L. B. Feldman (ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 109–129). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Templeton, S., & Scarborough-Franks, L. (1985). The spelling’s the thing: Knowledge of derivational morphology in orthography and phonology among older students. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 371–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, G. (1993). Wide range achievement test, 3rd edition. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Windsor, J. (2000). The role of phonological opacity in reading achievement. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 50–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. M. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery test - revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Carlisle, J.F., Stone, C.A. & Katz, L.A. The effects of phonological transparency on reading derived words. Ann. of Dyslexia 51, 249–274 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-001-0013-2

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-001-0013-2

Keywords

Navigation