Skip to main content
Log in

A cognitive theoretical approach to reading diagnostics

  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

After a brief review of current reading diagnostic practices, the article presents a cognitive developmental theory and indicates how the theory can be used to identify potential sources of reading problems. Four criteria for a reading diagnostic system based on cognitive developmental theory are then identified: (1) the system must provide reliable and valid assessments, (2) performance on the diagnostic system must be consistent with cognitive developmental theory, (3) the diagnostic procedures must provide specific information about the nature of the reading difficulty the student is experiencing, and (4) diagnoses provided by the procedures must lead to prescriptive procedures that alleviate to a demonstrable degree the reading problems the student is experiencing. The Computer-based Academic Assessment System (CAAS) is then introduced, and evidence from three studies is used to examine the extent to which the CAAS system can satisfy the four requirements for a theory-based reading diagnostic system. The article closes with a discussion of the empirical evidence and with suggestions for future research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aaron, P. G. (1991). Can reading disabilities be diagnosed without using intelligence tests?J. Learning Disab. 24(3): 178–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M. J. (1990).Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning About Print, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983).The Architecture of Cognition, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. C., and Pearson, P. D. (1984). A schema-theoretic view of basic processes in reading comprehension, In Pearson, P. D., Barr, R., and Kamil, M. (eds.),Handbook Reading Research, Longman, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. C., Reynolds, R. E., Shallert, D. L., and Goetz, E. T. (1977). Frameworks for comprehending discourse.Am. Educ. Res. J. 14: 367–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allington, R. L. (1984). Content coverage and contextual reading in reading groups.J. Reading Behav. 16: 85–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L., and Brown, A. L. (1984). Metacognitive skills in reading. In Pearson, P. D. (ed.),Handbook of Reading Research, Longman, New York, pp. 353–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ball, E. W. and Blachman, B. A. (1988). Phoneme segmentation training: Effect on reading readiness.Ann. Dyslexia 38: 208–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bejar, I. I. (1984). Educational diagnostic assessment.J. Educ. Measurement 21(2): 175–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A. (1977–1978). Relationship between oral reading rates for letters, words, and simple text in the development of reading achievement.Reading Res. Quart. 13: 223–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L. (1988). Rhyme recognition and reading and spelling in young children. In Ellis, W. (ed.),Intimacy with Language: A Forgotten Basic in Teacher Education (2nd ed.), Orton Dyslexia Society, Baltimore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read—A causal connection.Nature 30: 419–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. E. (1985).Rhyme and Reason in Reading and Spelling, University of Michigan Pres, Ann Arbor.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L., and Campione, J.C. (1986). Psychological theory and the study of learning disabilities.Am. Psychol. 41: 1059–1068.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E., and Bradley, L. (1985).Children's Reading Difficulties, Blackwell, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickley, A. C., Ellington, B. J., and Brickley, R. T. (1970). The cloze procedure: A conspectus.J. Reading Behav. 2: 232–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (1989a). The use of the Sentence Verification Technique in diagnostic assessment of listening and reading comprehension.Learning Disab. Res. 5: 33–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (1989b). Diagnosing comprehension deficits through listening and reading.Ann. Dyslexia 39: 159–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (1990). Diagnostic assessment of listening and reading comprehension. In Swanson, H. L., and Keogh, B. (eds.),Learning Disabilities: Theoretical and Research Issues, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chabot, R. J., Petros, T. V., and McCord, G. (1983). Developmental and reading ability differences in accessing information form semantic memory.J. Exp. Child Psychol. 35: 128–142.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cisero, C. A., Royer, J. M., Marchant, H., and Wint, F. (1994).Diagnostic Identification of Dyslexic and Learning Disabled College Students Using the Computer-Based Academic Assessment System (CAAS). Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Association, New Orleans, LA.

  • Cunningham, P. M. (1975–1976). Investigating a synthesized theory of mediated word identification.Reading Res. Quart. 11: 127–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, M. E. (1980). Development of components of reading skill.J. Educ. Psychol. 72: 656–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeSoto, J. L., and DeSoto, C. L. (1983). Relationship of reading achievement to verbal processing abilities.J. Educ. Psychol. 75: 116–127.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dewitt, P. A., Snyder, K., and Coressel, L. L. (1992).Comparing an Analogy and Phonics Approach to Word Recognition. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Reading Conference, San Antonio.

  • Doehring, D. G. (1976). Acquisition of rapid reading responses.Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 41 (2, of Serial No. 165).

  • Durrell, D. D., and Catterson, J. H. (1980).Durrell Analysis of Reading of Reading Difficulties, Psychological Corporation, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1991). Development of the ability to read words. In Barr, R., Kamil, M. L., Mosenthal, P. B., and Pearson, P. D. (eds.),Handbook of Reading Research (Vol. 2), Longman, New York, pp. 383–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L., and Treiman, R. (eds.),Reading Acquisition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 107–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983).The Modularity of the Mind, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I. (1979). Levels of processing and the structure of the language processor. In Cooper, W. E., and E. Walker, (eds.),Sentence Processing: Psycholinguistic Studies Presented to Merrill Garrett, Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, J. R., Warren, B. M., and Rosebery, A. S. (1985a). A componential approach to training reading skills: Part 1. Perceptual units training.Cognit. Instr. 2: 91–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, J. R., Warren, B. W., and Rosebery, A. S. (1985b). A componential approach to training reading skills: Part 2: Decoding and the use of context.Cognit. Instr. 2: 175–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaskins, R. W., Gaskins, J. C., and Gaskins, I. W. (1991). A decoding program for poor readers—and the rest of the class, too!Language Arts 68: 213–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, R. (1981). The future of testing: A research agenda for cognitive psychology and psychometrics.Am. Psychol. 36: 923–936.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glass, G. (1986). Testing old, testing new: Schoolboy psychology and the allocation of intellectual resources. In Plake, B. S., and Witt, J. C. (eds.),The Future of Testing, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 9–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, Y. M., and Burke, C. L. (1972).Reading Miscue Inventory, Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., and Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability.Remedial Spec. Educ. 7(1): 6–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., Juel, C., and Griffith, P. L. (1992). Reading, spelling, and the orthographic cipher. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L., and Treiman, R. (eds.),Reading Acquisition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 55–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, J. T. (1973). Models of reading and reading disability.J. Educ. Psychol. 65: 9–18.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J., and Humphreys, M. (1982). Research on specific learning disabilities: Deficits and remediation.Topics Learning Disab. 2: 68–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogaboam, T. W., and Perfetti, C. A. (1978). Reading skill and the role of verbal experience in decoding.J. Educ. Psychol. 70: 717–729.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E., Lunneborg, C., and Lewis, J. (1975). What does it mean to be high verbal?Cognit. Psychol. 7: 194–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, M. D., and McClelland, J. L. (1979). Processing determinants of reading speed.J. Exp. Psychol. General 108: 151–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, P. (1992).The Language of Assessment and the Assessment of Language. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Reading Conference, San Antonio, TX.

  • Johnston, P. H. (1984). Assessment in reading. In Pearson, P. D. (ed.),Handbook of Reading Research, Longman, New York, pp. 147–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., and Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading.Cognit. Psychol. 6: 293–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linn, R. L. (1986). Educational testing and assessment: Research needs and policy issues.Am. Psychol. 41: 1153–1160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Frost, J., and Peterson, O. P. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children.Reading Res. Quart. 23: 263–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mestre, J. P., and Royer, J. M. (1991). Cultural and linguistic influences on Latino testing. In Keller, G., Beneen, J., and Magallan, R. (eds.),Assessment and Access: Hispanics in Higher Education, State University of New York Press, Albany, NY, pp. 39–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, J. L. (1979).Fundamentals of Experimental Design (3rd Ed.), Allyn and Bacon, Boston, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., and Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English.Reading Res. Quart. 20: 233–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norusis, M. J. (1988).SPSS/PC + Advanced Statistics V2.0, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. K., and Wise, B. (1992). Reading on the computer with orthographic and speech feedback: An overview of the Colarado Remediation Project.Reading Writing, 4: 107–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palincsar, A. S., and Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and monitoring activities.Cogn. Instr. 1: 117–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, J., Macleod, C. M., Hunt, E., and Davidson, J. E. (1985). Information processing correlates of reading.J. Mem. Lang. 24: 59–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S. G., Cross, D. R., and Lipson, M. Y. (1984). Informed strategies for learning: A program to improve children's reading awareness and comprehension.J. Educ. Psychol. 76: 1239–1252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985).Reading Ability, Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1988). Verbal efficiency in reading ability. In Daneman, M., MacKinnon, G. E., and Waller, T. G. (eds.),Reading Research: Advances in Theory and Practice, Academic Press, New York, pp. 109–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L., and Treiman, R. (eds.),Reading Acquisition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 145–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., and Hogaboam, T. W. (1975). Relationship between single word decoding and reading comprehension skill.J. Educ. Psychol. 67: 461–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., and Lesgold, A. M. (1977). Discourse comprehension and sources of individual differences. In Just, M., and Carpenter, P. (eds.),Cognitive Processes in Comprehension, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A. Hewson, P. W., and Gertzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change.Sci. Educ. 66: 211–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Boies, S. J., Eichelman, W. H., and Taylor, R. L. (1969). Retention of physical and name codes of single letters.J. Exp. Psychol. Monogr. 79: 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., Goodchild, F., Fleet, J., Zajchowski, R., and Evans, E. D. (1989). The challenges of classroom strategy instruction.Elem. School J. 89: 301–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., and Pollatsek, A. (1989).The Psychology of Reading, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, S. F., and Beck, I. L. (1987). Theoretical and instructional implications of the assessment of two microcomputer word recognition programs.Reading Res. Quart. 22: 197–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royer, J. M. (1985). Reading from the perspective of a biological metaphor.Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 10: 150–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royer, J. M. (1990). The Sentence Verification Technique: A new direction in the assessment of reading comprehension. In Legg, S. M., and Algina, J. (eds.),Cognitive Assessment of Language and Math Outcomes, Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp. 144–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royer, J. M., and Carlo, M. S. (1991). Assessing the language acquisition progress of limited English proficient students: Problems and a new alternative.Appl. Measurement Educ. 4: 85–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royer, J. M., and Cunningham, D. J. (1981). On the theory and measurement of reading comprehension.Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 6: 187–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royer, J. M., Kulhavy, R. W., Lee, J. B., and Peterson, S. E. (1986). The sentence verification technique as a measure of listening and reading comprehension.Educ. Psychol. Res. 6: 299–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royer, J. M., Carlo, M. S., and Cisero, C. A. (1992). School-based uses for the Sentence Verification Technique of measuring listening and reading comprehension.Psychol. Test Bull 5: 5–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (1988). Micro experimental laboratory. An integrated system for IBM PC compatible.Behav. Res. Meth. Instr. Comp. 20: 206–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. (1980).Measuring Reading Competence: A Theoretical-Prescriptive Approach, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinatra, G. M., and Royer, J. M. (1993). The development of cognitive component processing skills that support skilled reading.J. Educ. Psychol. 85: 509–519.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, R. J. (1980). Constructive processes in prose comprehension and recall. In Spiro, R. J., Bruce, B. C., and Brewer, W. F. (eds.),Theoretical Issues in Reading Comprehension: Perspectives from Cognitive Psychology, Linguistics, Artificial Intelligence, and Education, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 245–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986a). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy.Reading Res. Quart. 21: 360–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986b). Cognitive processes and the reading problems of learning disabled children: Evaluating the assumption of specificity. In Torgesen, J., and Wong, B. (eds.),Psychological and Educational Perspectives on Learning Disabilities, Academic Press, New York, pp. 87–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the difference between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader. The phonological-core variable-difference model.J. Learning Disab. 21(10): 590–604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Concepts in developmental theories of reading skill: Cognitive resources, automaticity, and modularity,Dev. Rev. 10: 72–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stedman, L. C., and Kaestle, C. E. (1987). Literacy and reading performance in the United States from 1880 to the present.Reading Res. Quart. 22: 8–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1981). Testing and cognitive psychology.Am. Psychol. 36: 1181–1189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swinney, D. A. (1979). Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)Consideration of context effects.J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. 18: 645–659.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vinsonhaler, J. S., Weinshank, A. B., Wagner, C. C., and Polin, R. M. (1983). Diagnosing children with educational problems: Characteristics of reading and learning disabilities specialists, and classroom teachers.Reading Res. Quart. 28: 134–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Dijk, T. A., and Kintsch, W. (1983).Strategies of Discourse Comprehension, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagstaff, J. M., and Sinatra, G. M. (1994).Promoting Efficient and Independent Word Recognition: A New Strategy for Readers and Writers, Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Wade, S. E., Schraw, G., Buxton, W. M., and Hayes, M. T. (1993). Seduction of the strategic reader: Effects of interest on strategy and recall.Reading Res. Quart 28(2): 92–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walberg, H. J., and Tsai, S. (1983). Matthew effects in education.Am. Educ. Res. J. 20: 359–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. (1984). Naming, reading, and the dyslexias: A longitudinal review.Ann. Dyslexia 34: 78–115.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Royer, J.M., Sinatra, G.M. A cognitive theoretical approach to reading diagnostics. Educ Psychol Rev 6, 81–113 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02208969

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02208969

Key Words

Navigation