Skip to main content
Log in

Towards curricular coherence in integers and fractions: a study of the efficacy of a lesson sequence that uses the number line as the principal representational context

  • Original Article
  • Published:
ZDM Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper reports a study of the efficacy of Learning Mathematics through Representations (LMR), an innovative curriculum unit designed to support upper elementary students’ understandings of integers and fractions. The unit supports an integrated treatment of integers and fractions through (a) the use of the number line as a cross-domain representational context, and (b) the building of mathematical definitions in classroom communities that become resources to support student argumentation, generalization, and problem solving. In the efficacy study, fourth and fifth grade teachers employing the same district curriculum (Everyday Mathematics) were matched on background indicators and then assigned to either the LMR experimental classrooms (n = 11) or the comparison group (n = 8 with 10 classrooms). During the fall semester, LMR teachers implemented the LMR unit on 19 days and district curriculum on other days of mathematics instruction. HLM analyses documented greater achievement for LMR students than Comparison students on both the end-of-unit and the end-of year assessments of integers and fractions knowledge; the growth rates of LMR students were similar regardless of entering ability level, and gains for LMR students occurred on item types that included number line representations and those that did not. The findings point to the efficacy of the LMR sequence in supporting teaching and learning in the domains of integers and fractions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Two teachers taught mathematics to two classrooms.

  2. Two LMR teachers participated in the process of the curriculum’s iterative refinement. Though these two teachers (LMR-familiar) were more familiar with the LMR lessons than the other LMR teachers (LMR-new), analyses based on three participating groups (LMR-familiar, LMR-new, and Comparison) revealed that student learning gains in the LMR-familiar classrooms were not statistically different from gains in the LMR-new classrooms and indeed fell well within the range of the gains in other classrooms. We therefore included these two teachers in the LMR group for purposes of our efficacy analyses.

  3. We calculated the demographic statistics on 90 % of the participating students. We did not have access to this information for the remaining students.

  4. See the Appendix for greater detail on the measurement and longitudinal models.

  5. If we compare these findings to the pooled standard deviation of the test scores at pretest (1.2 logits), the differences between LMR and Comparison students were 1.3 and 0.7 pretest standard deviations at posttest and final test, respectively.

  6. Students in the Low pretest score group showed the most growth on the Integers subscale during the Integers lessons, and students in the Middle pretest score group showed the most growth on the Fractions subscale during the Fractions lessons.

  7. A random coefficient for the change from pretest to interim test was not included in the final model because its estimated variance was very close to zero and its inclusion did not improve overall model fit.

  8. As in the first model, a random coefficient for the change from pretest to interim test was not included in the final model.

References

  • Adams, R. J., Wilson, M., & Wang, W. C. (1997). The multidimensional random coefficients multinomial logit model. Applied Psychological Measurement, 21(1), 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, E. B. (1985). Estimating latent correlations between repeated testings. Psychometrika, 50, 3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J., & Kessel, C. (1999). Concept and computation: the role of curriculum. Mathematics and Education Reform Forum, 12(1), 4–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. E., & Clements, D. H. (2003). Quantifying path length: fourth-grade children’s developing abstractions for linear measurement. Cognition and Instruction, 21(4), 475–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bastable, V., Murray, M., Bloomfield, K., Russell, S. J., Schifter, D., Cochran, K., et al. (2012). Investigations in number, data, and space. Glenview: Scott Foresman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behr, M. J., Harel, G., Post, T., & Lesh, R. (1994a). Units of quantity: a conceptual basis common to additive and multiplicative structures. In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 121–176). Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behr, M. J., Harel, G., Post, T., & Lesh, R. (1994b). Units of quantity: a conceptual basis common to additive and multiplicative structures. In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 123–180). Albany: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bright, G., Behr, M., Post, T., & Wachsmuth, I. (1988). Identifying fractions on number lines. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 19(3), 215–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruno, A., & Martinon, A. (1999). The teaching of numerical extensions: the case of negative numbers. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 30(6), 789–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, D. H. (2007). Curriculum research: toward a framework for “research-based curricula”. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38(1), 35–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., Confrey, J., diSessa, A., Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2003). Design experiments in educational research. Educational Researcher, 32(1), 9–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., & Gravemeijer, K. (2008). Experimenting to support and understand learning processes. In A. E. Kelly, R. Lesh, & J. Y. Baek (Eds.), Handbook of design research methods in education: Innovations in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics learning and teaching (pp. 68–95). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Core State Standards Initiative (2010). The Standards: Mathematics. Common Core State Standards Initiative. http://www.corestandards.org/the-standards/mathematics.

  • Corwin, R. B., & TERC (1991). Seeing fractions: A unit for the upper elementary grades. Sacramento, California.

  • Davydov, V. V., & Tsvetkovich, Z. H. (1991). On the objective origin of the concept of fractions. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 13(1), 13–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fosnot, C. T., Dolk, M., Cameron, A., & Hersch, S. (2004). Young mathematicians at work: Professional development materials. Portsmough: Heinemann Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gearhart, M., & Saxe, G. B. (2012). Differentiated instruction in shared mathematical contexts. Teaching Children Mathematics (in press).

  • Izsak, A., Tillema, E., & Tunc-Pekkan, Z. (2008). Teaching and learning fraction addition on number lines. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 39(1), 33–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, P. A. (2000). Concrete representation and number line models: connecting and extending. Journal of Developmental Education, 24(2), 2–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleiman, G. M., & Bjork, E. D. (1991). My travels with Gulliver. Scotts Valley: Wings for Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, A. S., Beishuizen, M., & Treffers, A. (1998). The empty number line in Dutch second grades: realistic versus gradual program design. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29, 443–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R. (2003). Developing understanding of measurement. In J. Kilpatrick, W. G. Martin, & D. E. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to Principles and Standards for School Mathematics (pp. 179–192). Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2004). Modeling natural variation through distribution. American Educational Research Journal, 41(3), 635–679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mack, N. K. (1995). Confounding whole-number and fraction concepts when building on informal knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26(5), 422–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, M. R., Dekker, T., & Eade, F. (2010). Cross-national curriculum collaboration: Examples based on realistic mathematics education. In B. J. Reys, R. E. Reys, & R. Rubenstein (Eds.), Mathematics curriculum: Issues, trends, and future directions (pp. 145–156). Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murata, A. (2008). Mathematics teaching and learning as a mediating process: the case of tape diagrams. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 10, 374–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Center for Education Statistics. (2009). The Nation’s Report Card: Mathematics 2009 (NCES 2010–451). Washington: Institute of Education Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2006). Curriculum focal points for prekindergarten through grade 8 mathematics: A quest for coherence. Reston: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Mathematics Advisory Panel. (2008). The final report of the national mathematics advisory panel: Foundations for success. Washington: U.S. Department of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pastor, D. A., & Beretvas, S. N. (2006). Longitudinal Rasch modeling in the context of psychotherapy outcomes assessment. Applied Psychological Measurement, 30(2), 100–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petitto, A. L. (1990). Development of number line and measurement concepts. Cognition and Instruction, 7(1), 55–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1968). Quantification, conservation, and nativism. Science, 162(3857), 976–979.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., & Szeminska, A. (1960). The child’s conception of geometry (Trans: Lunzer, E.A.). New York: Basic Books.

  • R Development Corp Team. (2012). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramani, G. B., & Siegler, R. S. (2011). Reducing the gap in numerical knowledge between low- and middle-income preschoolers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 32(3), 146–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S., & Bryk, A. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. B., Earnest, D., Sitabkhan, Y., Haldar, L. C., Lewis, K. E., & Zheng, Y. (2010). Supporting generative thinking about the integer number line in elementary mathematics. Cognition & Instruction, 28(4), 433–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. B., Shaughnessy, M. M., Gearhart, M., & Haldar, L. C. (2012) Coordinating numerical and linear units: Elementary students’ strategies for locating whole numbers on the number line. Mathematics Teaching and Learning (in press).

  • Saxe, G. B., Shaughnessy, M. M., Shannon, A., Langer-Osuna, J., Chinn, R., & Gearhart, M. (2007). Learning about fractions as points on a number line NCTM Yearbook. Reston: NCTM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. B., Taylor, E. V., McIntosh, C., & Gearhart, M. (2005). Representing fractions with standard notation: A developmental analysis. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 36(2), 137–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, W. H., Whan, H. C., & McKnight, C. C. (2005). Curriculum coherence: an examination of US mathematics and science content standards from an international perspective. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 37(5), 525–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B. B., Kohn, A. S., & Resnick, L. B. (1993). Positives about negatives: a case study of an intermediate model for signed numbers. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 3, 37–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Selter, C. (1998). Building on children’s mathematics: a teaching experiment in grade three. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 36(1), 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2009). Playing linear number board games—but not circular ones—improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 545–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • StataCorp. (2011). Stata Statistical Software: Release 12. College Station: StataCorp LP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, J. W., Gonzales, P., Kawanaka, T., Knoll, S., & Serrano, A. (1999). The TIMSS videotape classroom study: Methods and findings from an exploratory research project on eighth-grade mathematics instruction in Germany, Japan, and the United States. Washington: U.S. Department of Education. National Center for Education Statistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • University of Chicago School Mathematics Project (2007). Everyday Mathematics (Third edition ed.): New York: McGraw-Hill Education.

  • Warm, T. A. (1989). Weighted likelihood estimation of ability in item response theory. Psychometrika, 54, 427–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe, T. (2007). Initial treatment of fractions in Japanese textbooks. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 29(2), 41–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe, T., Takahasi, A., & Yoshida, M. (2010). Supporting focused and cohesive curricula through visual representations: An example from Japanese textbooks. In B. J. Reys, R. E. Reys, & R. Rubenstein (Eds.), Mathematics curriculum: Issues, trends, and future directions (pp. 131–144). Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • What Works Clearinghouse. (2010a). Developing effective fractions instruction for kindergarten through 8th grade (No. NCEE 2010-4039). Washington: U.S. Department of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • What Works Clearinghouse. (2010b). Everyday Mathematics: WWC intervention report. Washington: U.S. Department of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M., Zheng, X., & McGuire, L. W. (2012). Formulating latent growth using an explanatory item response model approach. Journal of Applied Measurement, 13(1), 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, H. (2008). Fractions, decimals, and rational numbers. University of California, Berkeley. http://math.berkeley.edu/~wu/NMPfractions4.pdf. Accessed 23 July 2012.

  • Wu, M. L., Adams, R. J., Wilson, M. R., & Haldane, S. A. (2007). ACER ConQuest: Version 2.0. Victoria: ACER Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yackel, E., & Cobb, P. (1996). Sociomathematical norms, argumentation, and authonmy in mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27(4), 458–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The research reported here was supported in part by the Institute of Education Sciences grant R305B070299 and R305B090026 to UC Berkeley. The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views of the Institute of Education Sciences or the U.S. Department of Education. We are grateful to the participating teachers and students and to the many contributors to the project. Rick Kleine and Jenn Pfotenhauer served as collaborating teachers in the development of the lesson sequence. A team of doctoral students contributed to the research and to curriculum development: Darrell Earnest, Lina Chopra Haldar, Bona Kang, Katherine Lewis, and Yasmin Sitabkhan. Additional contributors include Nicole Leveille Buchanan, Jennifer Collett, Kenton de Kirby, David Torres Irribarra, Marie Le, Amanda McKerracher, Meghan Shaughnessy, Alison Miller Singley, Josh Sussman, and Ying Zheng. We are grateful to Professors Deborah Loewenberg Ball and Hyman Bass for feedback on the curriculum. Email correspondence: saxe@berkeley.edu.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Geoffrey B. Saxe.

Appendix

Appendix

1.1 Measurement model

The measurement model used to obtain student ability estimates is given by:

$$ {\text{logit}}\left( {X_{sti} = 1\left| {{\varvec{\theta}},{\varvec{\delta}}} \right.} \right) = \theta_{st} - \delta_{i} $$

where \( X_{sti} \) indicates the response of student s at time t is for item i, \( \theta_{st} \) is the person and time specific ability, and \( \delta_{i} \) is the item difficulty. The \( \varvec{\theta}_{s} \) are assumed to follow a multivariate normal distribution with unrestricted covariance matrix. All measurement models were fit using ConQuest (Wu, Adams, Wilson, & Haldane, 2007). Table 5 contains the estimated correlation matrix between the assessments at the four occasions for the overall scale.

Table 5 Correlations between the assessments (from the estimated covariance matrix of the item response model)

1.2 Longitudinal growth models

Three longitudinal growth models were estimated using Stata12 (StataCorp. 2011) and graphed using R (R Development Corp Team, 2012).

1.2.1 Model 1

The first model, used for the overall comparison between LMR and Comparison students, is specified in three levels as follows:

Level 1:

$$ {\text{wle}}_{tsc} = \pi_{0sc} + \pi_{1sc} {\text{inter}}_{tsc} + \pi_{2sc} {\text{post}}_{tsc} + \pi_{3sc} {\text{final}}_{tsc} + \varepsilon_{tsc} $$

Level 2:

$$ \begin{aligned} \pi_{psc} &= \beta_{p0c} + \zeta_{psc} \quad {\text{for}}\quad p = 0,2,3 \\ \pi_{1sc} &= \beta_{10c} \\ \end{aligned} $$

Level 3:

$$ \begin{aligned} \beta_{00c} &= \gamma_{000} + \gamma_{001} {\text{comp}}_{c} + \zeta_{c} \\ \beta_{10c} & = \gamma_{100} \\ \beta_{p0c} & = \gamma_{p00} + \gamma_{p01} {\text{comp}}_{c} \quad {\text{for}}\quad p = 2,3 \\ \end{aligned} $$

\( wle_{tsc} \) is the WLE estimate of overall student ability for student s in classroom c at time t and \( inter_{tsc} \), \( post_{tsc} \), and \( final_{tsc} \) are indicators for test occasion. \( comp_{c} \) is an indicator for classroom c being in the comparison group. \( \zeta_{c} \) is a random classroom-level effect assumed to follow a normal distribution. The \( \zeta_{sc} \) are student-level random effects assumed to follow a multivariate normal distribution with unrestricted covariance matrix.Footnote 7 \( \varepsilon_{tsc} \) is a time-specific error assumed to follow a normal distribution.

Table 6 gives the estimated variance components from fitting this model to the data. Table 1 in the text gives a subset of the estimated parameters (and their linear combinations). Figure 9 was drawn using estimates from this model.

Table 6 Estimated variance components from Model 1

1.2.2 Model 2

The second model extends the first model to include ability estimates for all subscales as the response variable and indicator variables for the four subscales (with the overall scale as the reference category), as follows:

Level 1:

$$ \begin{aligned} {\text{wle}}_{tsc} &= \pi_{0sc} + \pi_{1sc} {\text{inter}}_{tsc} + \pi_{2sc} {\text{post}}_{tsc} + \pi_{3sc} {\text{final}}_{tsc} \\ & \quad + \pi_{4sc} {\text{integers}}_{tsc} + \pi_{5sc} {\text{fractions}}_{tsc} + \pi_{6sc} {\text{line}}_{tsc} + \pi_{7sc} {\text{noline}}_{tsc} \\ & \quad + \pi_{8sc} {\text{inter}}_{tsc} \times {\text{integers}}_{tsc} + \pi_{9sc} {\text{inter}}_{tsc} \times {\text{fractions}}_{tsc} + \pi_{10sc} {\text{inter}}_{tsc} \times {\text{line}}_{tsc} + \pi_{11sc} {\text{inter}}_{tsc} \times {\text{noline}}_{tsc} \\ & \quad + \pi_{12sc} {\text{post}}_{tsc} \times {\text{integers}}_{tsc} + \pi_{13sc} {\text{post}}_{tsc} \times {\text{fractions}}_{tsc} + \pi_{14sc} {\text{post}}_{tsc} \times {\text{line}}_{tsc} + \pi_{15sc} {\text{post}}_{tsc} \times {\text{noline}}_{tsc} \\ & \quad + \pi_{16sc} {\text{final}}_{tsc} \times {\text{integers}}_{tsc} + \pi_{17sc} {\text{final}}_{tsc} \times {\text{fractions}}_{tsc} + \pi_{18sc} {\text{final}}_{tsc} \times {\text{line}}_{tsc} + \pi_{19sc} {\text{final}}_{tsc} \times {\text{noline}}_{tsc} \\ & \quad + \varepsilon_{tsc} \\ \end{aligned} $$

Level 2:

$$ \begin{aligned} \pi_{psc} &= \beta_{p0c} + \zeta_{psc} \quad {\text{for}}\quad p = 0,1,2,3 \\ \pi_{psc} & = \beta_{p0c} \quad \quad \quad \;\;{\text{for}}\quad p = 4,5, \ldots ,19 \\ \end{aligned} $$

Level 3:

$$ \begin{aligned} \beta_{00c} &= \gamma_{000} + \gamma_{001} {\text{comp}}_{c} + \zeta_{c} \\ \beta_{p0c} &= \gamma_{p00} \quad \quad \quad \quad \quad \quad \quad {\text{for}}\quad p = 1,8,9,10,11 \\ \beta_{p0c} & =\gamma_{p00} + \gamma_{p01} {\text{comp}}_{c} \quad \quad {\text{for}}\quad p = 2, \ldots ,7,12, \ldots ,19 \\ \end{aligned} $$

\( wle_{tsc} \) is the WLE estimate of overall student ability for student s in classroom c at time t, \( inter_{tsc} \), \( post_{tsc} \), and \( final_{tsc} \) are indicators for test occasion, and \( integers_{tsc} \), \( fractions_{tsc} \), \( line_{tsc} \), and \( noline_{tsc} \) are indicators for subscale. \( comp_{c} \) is an indicator for classroom c being in the comparison group. \( \zeta_{c} \) is a random classroom-level effect assumed to follow a normal distribution. The student-level random effects \( \zeta_{sc} \) are assumed to follow a multivariate normal distribution with unrestricted covariance matrix. \( \varepsilon_{tsc} \) is a time-specific error assumed to follow a normal distribution.

Table 7 presents the estimated variance components from fitting this model to the data. Tables 2, 3, and 4 (in the text) contain a subset of the estimated parameters (and their linear combinations). Figures 11 and 13 are drawn using estimates from this model.

Table 7 Estimated variance components from Model 2

1.2.3 Model 3

The third model extends the first model to include indicator variables for student pretest score group (with the low group as the reference category), as follows:

Level 1:

$$ {\text{wle}}_{tsc} = \pi_{0sc} + \pi_{1sc} {\text{inter}}_{tsc} + \pi_{2sc} {\text{post}}_{tsc} + \pi_{3sc} {\text{final}}_{tsc} + \varepsilon_{tsc} $$

Level 2:

$$ \begin{aligned} \pi_{psc} &= \beta_{p0c} + \beta_{p1c} {\text{med}}_{sc} + \beta_{p2c} {\text{high}}_{sc} + \zeta_{psc} \quad {\text{for}}\quad p = 0,2,3 \\ \pi_{1sc} &= \beta_{10c} + \beta_{p1c} {\text{med}}_{sc} + \beta_{p2c} {\text{high}}_{sc} \\ \end{aligned} $$

Level 3:

$$ \begin{aligned} \beta_{00c} &= \gamma_{000} + \gamma_{001} {\text{comp}}_{c} + \zeta_{c} \\ \beta_{0qc} &= \gamma_{0q0} + \gamma_{0q1} {\text{comp}}_{c} \quad \quad {\kern 1pt} {\text{for}}\quad q = 1,2 \\ \beta_{1qc} & = \gamma_{1q0} \quad \quad \quad \quad \quad \quad \quad \;{\text{for}}\quad q = 0,1,2 \\ \beta_{pqc} &= \gamma_{pq0} + \gamma_{pq1} {\text{comp}}_{c} \quad \quad {\text{for}}\quad p = 2,3\;and\;q = 0,1,2 \\ \end{aligned} $$

\( wle_{tsc} \) is the WLE estimate of student achievement on the scale of interest for student s in classroom c at time t; this model was fit using the overall subscale and then the integers and fractions subscales as the dependent variable. \( inter_{tsc} \), \( post_{tsc} \), and \( final_{tsc} \) are indicators for test occasion. \( med_{sc} \) and \( high_{sc} \) are indicator variables for a student being in the medium or high pretest score group, respectively. \( comp_{c} \) is an indicator for classroom c being in the comparison group. \( \zeta_{c} \) is a random classroom-level effect assumed to follow a normal distribution. The \( \zeta_{sc} \) are student-level random effects assumed to follow a multivariate normal distribution with unrestricted covariance matrix.Footnote 8 \( \varepsilon_{tsc} \) is a time-specific error assumed to follow a normal distribution.

Table 8 gives the estimated variance components from fitting this model to the data. Figures 10 and 12 are drawn using estimates from this model.

Table 8 Estimated variance components from Model 3 for different scales as dependent variable

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Saxe, G.B., Diakow, R. & Gearhart, M. Towards curricular coherence in integers and fractions: a study of the efficacy of a lesson sequence that uses the number line as the principal representational context. ZDM Mathematics Education 45, 343–364 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-012-0466-2

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-012-0466-2

Keywords

Navigation