Skip to main content
Log in

Representational disfluency in algebra: evidence from student gestures and speech

  • Original Article
  • Published:
ZDM Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this investigation, we analyzed US middle school students’ (grades 6–8) gestures and speech during interviews to understand students’ reasoning while interpreting quantitative patterns represented by Cartesian graphs. We studied students’ representational fluency, defined as their abilities to work within and translate among representations. While students translated across representations to address task demands, they also translated to a different representation when reaching an impasse, where the initial representation could not be used to answer a task. During these impasse events, which we call representational disfluencies, three categories of behavior were observed. Some students perceived the graph to be bounded by its physical and numerical limits, and these students were categorized as physically grounded. A second, related, disfluency was categorized as spatially grounded. Students who were classified as spatially grounded exhibited a bounded view of the graph that limited their ability to make far predictions until they physically altered the spatial configuration of the graph by rescaling or extending the axes. Finally, students who recovered from one or more of these disfluencies by translating the quantitative information to alternative but equivalent representations (i.e., exhibiting representational fluency), while retaining the connection back to the linear pattern as graphed, were categorized as interpretatively grounded. Understanding the causes and varieties of representational fluency and disfluency contributes directly to our understanding of mathematics knowledge, learning and adaptive forms of reasoning. These findings also provide implications for mathematics instruction and assessment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Another possible interpretation of Sophia’s use of the word “it” in line 9 is that she is referring to the graph as a whole unit, and views either the graph or the points plotted as malleable and organic.

References

  • Alibali, M. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1993). Gesture-speech mismatch and mechanisms of learning: What the hands reveal about a child’s state of mind. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 468–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beichner, R. (1994). Testing student interpretation of kinematics graphs. American Journal of Physics, 62, 750–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. B., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1986). The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition, 23(1), 43–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J. (1985). Misconceptions in graphing. Proceedings of the ninth conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, 1, 369–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, E. M. (1996). Gestures at work in sense-making science talk. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 5(3), 173–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curcio, F. R. (1987). Comprehension of mathematical relationships expressed in graphs. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 18(5), 382–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeBok, D., Verschaffela, L., Janssensc, D., Van Doorena, W., & Claesa, K. (2003). Do realistic contexts and graphical representations always have a beneficial impact on students’ performance? Negative evidence from a study on modelling non-linear geometry problems. Learning and instruction, 13(4), 441–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friel, S. N., Curcio, F. R., & Bright, G. W. (2001). Making sense of graphs: Critical factors influencing comprehension and instructional implications. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 32(2), 124–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerofsky, S. (2007). Traces of the body: Gestures and graphing in school mathematics. In L. Edwards, S. Gerofsky, & T. Noble (Eds.), Panel presentation, Gestures for thinking in mathematics and science. International Society for Gesture Studies Conference, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL.

  • Glaser, B. G. (1978). Theoretical sensitivity: Advances in the methodology of grounded theory. Mill Valley: Sociology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., De Vega, M., & Graesser, A. C. (2008). Framing the debate. In M. DeVega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols, Embodiment and Meaning (pp. 1–10). Cambridge: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Jaworski, B., Rischal, M, & Levin, J. R. (2009). What brains are for: Action, meaning, and reading comprehension. In D. McNamara (Ed.), Reading Comprehension Strategies: Theories, Interventions, and Technologies. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers. (in press)

  • Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (1999). Indexical understanding of instructions. Discourse Processes, 28, 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, R. L., & Son, J. Y. (2005). The transfer of scientific principles using concrete and idealized simulations. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14, 69–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1994). Professional vision. American Anthropologist, 96(3), 606–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, S., Case, R., & Siegler, R. S. (1994). Rightstart: Providing the central conceptual prerequisites for first formal learning of arithmetic to students at risk for school failure. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice (pp. 25–49). Cambridge: MIT Press/Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42, 335–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, J. A., Sloutsky, V. M., & Heckler, A. F. (2008). The advantage of abstract examples in learning math. Science, 320, 454–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Nunez, R. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the Embodied Mind Brings Mathematics into Being. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lannon, J. K., Barker, D. D., & Townsend, B. E. (2006). Recursive and explicit rules: how can we build student algebraic understanding? Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 25, 299–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lappan, G., Fey, J. T., Fitzgerald, W. M., Friel, S. N., & Phillips, E. D. (1998). Connected Mathematics Project. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leinhardt, G., Zaslavsky, O., & Stein, M. K. (1990). Functions, graphs, and graphing: Tasks, learning, and teaching. Review of Educational Research, 60(1), 1–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nathan, M. J. (2008). An embodied cognition perspective on symbols, gesture and grounding instruction. In M. DeVega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols, embodiment and meaning: a debate (pp. 375–396). Cambridge: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nathan, M. J., & Kim, S. (2007). Pattern generalization with graphs and words: A cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis of middle school students’ representational fluency. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 9(3), 193–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nathan, M. J., Kintsch, W., & Young, E. (1992). A theory of algebra word problem comprehension and its implications for the design of computer learning environments. Cognition and Instruction, 9(4), 329–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Council for Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pape, S., & Tschoshanov, M. A. (2001). The role of representation(s) in developing mathematical understanding. Theory Into Practice, 40(2), 118–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1990). A theory of graph comprehension. In R. Freedle (Ed.), Artificial Intelligence and the future of testing (pp. 73–126). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2009). ‘‘No! He starts walking backwards!’’: interpreting motion graphs and the question of space, place and distance. ZDM. doi:10.1007/s11858-009-0173-9.

  • RAND Mathematics Study Panel. (2003). Mathematics proficiency for all students: Toward a strategic research and development program in mathematics education. Santa Monica: RAND.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2001). Gestures: Their role in teaching and learning. Review of Educational Research, 71, 365–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W-M, G. Bowen, M., & McGinn, M. K. (1999). Differences in graph-related practices between high school biology textbooks and scientific ecology journals. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 36(9), 977–1019.

  • Saxe, G. B., Taylor, E. V., McIntosh, C., & Gearhart, M. (2005). Representing fractions with standard notation: a developmental analysis. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 36(2), 137–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. P., di Sessa, A. A., & Rochelle, J. (1993). Misconceptions reconceived: a constructivist analysis of knowledge in transition. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 3(2), 115–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. (2001). Spatial schemas in depictions. In M. Gattis (Ed.), Spatial schemas and abstract thought (pp. 79–112). Boston: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woods, D., & Fassnacht, C. (2007). Transana version 2.2. [Computer software]. Available from http://www.transana.org.

  • Zhang, J. (1997). The nature of external representations in problem solving. Cognitive Science, 21(2), 179–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J., & Norman, D. A. (1994). Representations in distributed cognitive tasks. Cognitive Science, 18(1), 87–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was funded in part by a grant entitled “Understanding and Cultivating the Transition from Arithmetic to Algebraic Reasoning,” awarded to the second author by the Interagency Educational Research Initiative, an alliance of the National Science Foundation, the US Department of Education’s Institute of Educational Sciences, and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development within the National Institutes of Health. We extend our appreciation to Andrew Garfield for his technical knowledge and assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kristen N. Bieda.

Appendices

Appendix A: Students’ pattern generalization strategy codes

See Table 4.

Table 4  

Appendix B: Transcripts of student responses

Gesture events coincide with boldface text and are described in parenthetical statements.

figure afigure a

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bieda, K.N., Nathan, M.J. Representational disfluency in algebra: evidence from student gestures and speech. ZDM Mathematics Education 41, 637–650 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-009-0198-0

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-009-0198-0

Keywords

Navigation