Skip to main content
Log in

Talking to children about science is harder than we think: characteristics and metacognitive judgments of explanations provided to children and adults

  • Published:
Metacognition and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Adults modify the way they speak to children to support children’s learning across several domains. However, no previous research has studied whether adults change their language when explaining science to children. The current study examined if and how adults change the manner in which they talk about science when providing explanations to children vs. providing explanations to other adults. Participants (N = 81) were video recorded while explaining basic science concepts to children and adults. Recordings were later analyzed to determine if and how participants changed the quality and content of their explanations. The results confirmed that adults did change their explanations when talking to children about science by providing more potentially beneficial, but also disadvantageous, information. Participants perceived that they provided more accurate explanations to children, but appeared to be making metacognitive judgments largely based upon the changes made that could be beneficial to learning. Taken together, this work suggests that science may be a domain in which adults are not well equipped to modify and monitor their language to children.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In Experiments 1 & 2, multivariate analyses revealed that the individual explanation characteristics did not predict perceived accuracy above and beyond all the other explanation characteristics.

References

  • Allen, S. (2002). Looking for learning in visitor talk: A methodological exploration. In G. Leinhardt, K. Crowley, & K. Knutson (Eds.), Learning conversations in museums (pp. 259–303). Malwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asoko, H. (2002). Developing conceptual understanding in primary science. Cambridge Journal of Education, 32(2), 153–164. doi:10.1080/03057640220147522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, L. A., Danovitch, J. H., & Keil, F. C. (2008). Children’s sensitivity to circular explanations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100(2), 146–155. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2007.10.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, L., Schachter, M., Winkler, A., & Wolfe, S. (2002). Concepts and Challenges in Life, Earth, and Physical Science. Globe/Pearson Publishing.

  • Blewitt, P. (1983). Dog versus collie: vocabulary in speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 19, 602–609. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.19.4.602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32(7), 513–531. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.32.7.513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Browne, C. A., & Woolley, J. D. (2004). Preschoolers' magical explanations for violations of physical, social, and mental laws. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5(2), 239–260. doi:10.1207/s15327647jcd0502_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1996). The culture of education. Harvard University Press.

  • Callanan, M. A., & Oakes, L. M. (1992). Preschoolers' questions and parents' explanations: Causal thinking in everyday activity. Cognitive Development, 7(2), 213–233. doi:10.1016/0885-2014(92)90012-G.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canfield, C. F., & Ganea, P. A. (2014). ‘You could call it magic’: What parents and siblings tell preschoolers about unobservable entities. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(2), 269–286. doi:10.1080/15248372.2013.777841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrier, S. J. (2013). Elementary preservice teachers’ science vocabulary: Knowledge and application. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 24(2), 405–425. doi:10.1007/s10972-012-9270-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chang, A., Sandhofer, C. M., Adelchanow, L., & Rottman, B. (2011). Parental numeric language input to mandarin Chinese and English speaking preschool children. Journal of Child Language, 38, 341–355. doi:10.1017/S0305000909990390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., deLeeuw, N., Chiu, M., & LaVancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding. Cognitive Science, 18, 439–477. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1803_3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61, 1584–1595. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02885.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowley, K., Callanan, M. A., Jipson, J. L., Galco, J., Topping, K., & Shrager, J. (2011). Shared scientific thinking in everyday parent-child activity. Science Education, 85(6), 712–732. doi:10.1002/sce.1035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowley, K., Callanan, M. A., Tenenbaum, H. R., & Allen, E. (2001). Parents explain more often to boys than to girls during shared scientific thinking. Psychological Science, 12(3), 258–261. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dagher, Z. R. (1995). Review of studies on the effectiveness of instructional analogies in science education. Science Education, 79(3), 295–312. doi:10.1002/sce.3730790305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 105–225. doi:10.1080/07370008.1985.9649008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durkin, K., Shire, B., Riem, R., Crowther, R. D., & Rutter, D. R. (1986). The social and linguistic context of early number word use. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(3), 269–288. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1986.tb01018.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fang, Z. (2006). The language demands of science reading in middle school. International Journal of Science Education, 28(5), 491–520. doi:10.1080/09500690500339092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fender, J. G., & Crowley, K. (2007). How parent explanation changes what children learn from everyday scientific thinking. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 189–210. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2007.02.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, C. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. American Anthropologist, 66, 103–114. doi:10.1525/aa.1964.66.suppl_3.02a00060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R., & Fromhoff, F. A. (1988). Style and structure in mother-child conversations about the past. Discourse Processes, 11, 337–355. doi:10.1080/01638538809544707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haden, C. A. (2010). Talking about science in museums. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 62–67. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00119.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L., & Koenig, M. A. (2006). Trust in testimony: how children learn about science and religion. Child Development, 77(3), 505–524. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00886.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarvie, I. C., & Agassi, J. (1967). The problem of the rationality of magic. British Journal of Sociology, 55-74.

  • Kallery, M., & Psillos, D. (2004). Anthropomorphism and animism in early years science: Why teachers use them, how they conceptualise them and what are their views on their use. Research in Science Education, 34(3), 291–311. doi:10.1023/B:RISE.0000044613.64634.03.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalish, C. W. (1996). Preschoolers' understanding of germs as invisible mechanisms. Cognitive Development, 11(1), 83–106. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(96)90029-5

  • Keil, F. C., & Wilson, R. A. (2000). Explanation and cognition. MIT Press.

  • Kemler Nelson, D. G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Jusczyk, P. W., & Wright Cassidy, K. (1989). How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning. Journal of Child Language, 16, 55–68. doi:10.1017/S030500090001343X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1993). How do we know that we know? the accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. Psychological Review, 100, 609–639. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legare, C. H., Lane, J. D., & Evans, E. M. (2013). Anthropomorphizing science: how does it affect the development of evolutionary concepts? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 59(2), 168–197. doi:10.1353/mpq.2013.0009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombrozo, T. (2006). The structure and function of explanations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 464–470. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., & Jackson, J. (2005). The case for coherence in scientific explanations: quantitative details can hurt qualitative understanding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11, 13–18. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.11.1.13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penno, J. F., Wilkinson, I. A., & Moore, D. W. (2002). Vocabulary acquisition from teacher explanation and repeated listening to stories: Do they overcome the Matthew effect? Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(1), 23–33. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.1.23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother-child conversations about the past: relationships of style and memory over time. Cognitive Development, 8, 403–430. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(05)80002-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rips, L. J. (2002). Circular reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26(6), 767–795. doi:10.1016/S0364-0213(02)00085-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rottman, B. M., & Keil, F. C. (2011). What matters in scientific explanations: effects of elaboration and content. Cognition, 121, 324–337. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russ, R. S. (2014). Epistemology of science vs. epistemology for science. Science Education, 98(3), 388–396. doi:10.1002/sce.21106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmon, W. (1984). Scientific explanation and the causal structure of the world. Princeton University Press.

  • Saxe, G. B., Guberman, S. R., Gearhart, M., Gelman, R., Massey, C. M., & Rogoff, B. (1987). Social processes in early number development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, i-162. doi:10.2307/1166071

  • Schwartz, B. L., & Metcalfe, J. (1992). Cue familiarity but not target retrievability enhances feeling-of-knowing judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1074–1083. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.5.1074.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipley, E., Kuhn, L., & Madden, C. (1983). Mother’s use of superordinate category terms. Journal of Child Language, 10, 571–588. doi:10.1017/S0305000900005377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stavy, R., & Tirosh, D. (2000). How students (mis-) understand science and mathematics: Intuitive rules. Teachers College Press.

  • Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 123–138. doi:10.1007/s10648-010-9128-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szechter, L. E., & Carey, E. J. (2009). Gravitating toward science: parent–child interactions at a gravitational-wave observatory. Science Education, 93(5), 846–858. doi:10.1002/sce.20333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tare, M., French, A., Frazier, B. N., Diamond, J., & Evans, E. M. (2011). Explanatory parent–child: conversation predominates at an evolution exhibit. Science Education, 95(4), 720–744. doi:10.1002/sce.20433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thagard, P. (1992). Analogy, explanation, and education. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 29(6), 537–544. doi:10.1002/tea.3660290603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant‐directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy, 7(1), 53–71. doi:10.1207/s15327078in0701_5.

  • Valle, A., & Callanan, M. A. (2006). Similarity comparisons and relational analogies in parent-child conversations about science topics. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52(1), 96–124. doi:10.1353/mpq.2006.0009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vendetti, M. S., Matlen, B. J., Richland, L. E., & Bunge, S. A. (2015). Analogical reasoning in the classroom: insights from cognitive science. Mind, Brain, and Education, 9(2), 100–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vlach, H. A., & Sandhofer, C. M. (2012). Distributing learning over time: the spacing effect in children’s acquisition and generalization of science concepts. Child Development, 83, 1137–1144. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01781.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge: MA. MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: MA. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wales, R., Coleman, M., & Pattison, P. (1983). How a thing is called: A study of mothers’ and children’s naming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 1–17. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(83)90053-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weisberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E., & Gray, J. R. (2008). The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 470–477. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., Pons, F., Dietrich, C., Kajikawa, S., Fais, L., & Amano, S. (2007). Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learning in English and Japanese. Cognition, 103, 147–162. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.03.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yager, R. E. (1983). The importance of terminology in teaching K-12 science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 20(6), 577–588. doi:10.1002/tea.3660200610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to the undergraduate research assistants of the UW-Madison Learning, Cognition, & Development Lab for their help with this project. We would also like to thank the Wisconsin Alumni Research Foundation for funding portions of this project.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Haley A. Vlach.

Ethics declarations

Funding

This study was funded the Wisconsin Alumni Research Foundation (Grant MSN156316).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vlach, H.A., Noll, N. Talking to children about science is harder than we think: characteristics and metacognitive judgments of explanations provided to children and adults. Metacognition Learning 11, 317–338 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-016-9153-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-016-9153-y

Keywords

Navigation