Skip to main content
Log in

Conceptual Change from the Framework Theory Side of the Fence

  • Published:
Science & Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We describe the main principles of the framework theory approach to conceptual change and briefly report on the results of a text comprehension study that investigated some of the hypotheses that derive from it. We claim that children construct a naive physics which is based on observation in the context of lay culture and which forms a relatively coherent conceptual system—i.e., a framework theory—that can be used as a basis for explanation and prediction of everyday phenomena. Learning science requires fundamental ontological, epistemological, and representational changes in naive physics. These conceptual changes take a long time to be achieved, giving rise to fragmentation and synthetic conceptions. We also argue that both fragmentation and synthetic conceptions can be explained to result from learners’ attempts assimilate scientific information into their existing but incompatible naive physics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See for example Vosniadou and Brewer (1992, 1994), Vosniadou and Skopeliti (2005), Vosniadou et al. (2004, 2005), Samarapungavan et al. (1996).

  2. Baillargeon (1995), Carey (2009), Carey and Spelke (1994), Gelman (1991).

  3. See, for example, Nussbaum (1979), Nussbaum and Novak (1976), Sneider and Pulos (1983), Vosniadou and Brewer (1992, 1994).

  4. See for example Mason and Gava (2007), Mason et al. (2008), Stathopoulou and Vosniadou (2007a, b), Kyriakopoulou and Vosniadou (2012).

  5. There are a number of other attempts to reconcile the framework theory approach with the knowledge-in-pieces approach, such as those described by Brown and Hammer (2013) and Clark and Linn (2013).

  6. Some of the other reasons are differences in methodology, data collection and scoring criteria (see also Clark and Linn 2013).

  7. See Stafylidou and Vosniadou (2004), Vamvakoussi and Vosniadou (2010, 2012) but also Ni and Zhou (2005), Van Hoof, et al. (in press).

  8. See for example Nussbaum (1979), Nussbaum and Novak (1976), Sadler (1987), Samarapungavan et al. (1996), Sneider and Pulos (1983), Vosniadou and Brewer (1994), Vosniadou et al. (2004).

  9. Group or class discussion and collaboration are often implicit uses of cognitive conflict.

References

  • Baillargeon, R. (1995). A model of physical reasoning in infancy. In C. Rovee-Collier & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (vol. 9, pp. 305–371). USA: Ablex.

  • Brown, D., & Hammer, D. (2013). Conceptual change in physics. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), The International handbook of conceptual change (2nd ed., pp. 121–137). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chan, C., Burtis, J., & Bereiter, C. (1997). Knowledge building as a mediator of conflict in conceptual change. Cognition and Instruction, 15, 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (1992). Conceptual change within and across ontological categories: Examples from learning and discovery in science. In R. Giere (Ed.), Cognitive models of science: Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science (pp. 129–186). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2008). Three types of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation, and categorical shift. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 61–82). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2013). Two kinds and four sub-types of misconceived knowledge, ways to change it, and the learning outcomes. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), The international handbook of conceptual change (2nd ed., pp. 49–70). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Slotta, J. D., & de Leeuw, N. (1994). From things to processes: A theory of conceptual change for learning science concepts. Learning and Instruction, 4, 27–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: A theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 63, 1–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C. A., & Malhotra, B. A. (2001). Epistemologically authentic scientific reasoning. In K. Crowley, C. D. Schunn, & T. Okada (Eds.), Designing for science: Implications from everyday, classroom, and professional settings (pp. 351–392). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C. A., & Malhotra, B. A. (2002). Epistemologically authentic reasoning in schools: A theoretical framework for evaluating inquiry tasks. Science Education, 86, 175–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. B., D’Angelo, C., & Schleigh, S. (2011). Comparison of students’ knowledge structure coherence and understanding of force in the Philippines, Turkey, China, Mexico, and the United States. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 20(2), 207–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D., & Linn, M. (2013). Promoting conceptual change through inquiry. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), The international handbook of conceptual change (2nd ed., pp. 520–538). New York: Routledge.

  • Clement, J. (2008). The role of explanatory models in teaching for conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), The international handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 417–452). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10(2/3), 105–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A. (2008). A bird’s-eye view of the “pieces” vs. “coherence” controversy (from the “pieces” side of the fence). In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 35–60). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A., Gillespie, N., & Esterly, J. (2004). Coherence vs. fragmentation in the development of the concept of force. Cognitive Science, 28, 843–900.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duschl, R. A., Schweingruber, H. A., & Shouse, A. W. (Eds.). (2007). Taking science to school: Learning and teaching science in grades K-8. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R. (1991). Epigenetic foundations of knowledge structures: Initial and transcendent constructions. In S. Carey & R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 293–322). Erlabaum: Hilldale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guzzetti, B. J., & Glass, G. V. (1993). Promoting conceptual change in science: A comparative meta-analysis of instructional interventions from reading education and science education. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 116–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (2003). When is conceptual change intended? A cognitive-sociocultural view. In G. M. Sinatra & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Intentional conceptual change (pp. 407–427). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2003). Conceptual and linguistic factors in inductive projection: How do young children recognize commonalities between animals and plants? In D. Genter & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language and thought in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 313–333). Cambridge, ΜΑ: ΜΙΤ Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ioannides, C., & Vosniadou, S. (2002). The changing meanings of force. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 2(1), 5–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ioannidou, I., & Vosniadou, S. (2001). The development of knowledge about the composition and layering of the earth’s interior. Nea Paedia, 31, (in Greek), 107–150.

  • Jonassen, D. (2008). Model building for conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 676–693). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1981). Constraints on knowledge and cognitive development. Psychological Review, 88, 197–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kyriakopoulou, N., & Vosniadou, S. (2012). The Relation between conceptual change in physical science, theory of mind and personal epistemology and implication for science instruction. Paper presented at the 8th international conference on conceptual change. 1–4 September, Trier, Germany.

  • Kyriakopoulou, N. & Vosniadou, S. (this volume). Using theory of mind to promote conceptual change in science, science & education.

  • Kyrkos, C., & Vosniadou, S. (1997). Mental models of plant nutrition: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Poster presented at the Seventh European Conference for Research on Learning and Instruction, Athens, Greece.

  • Lehrer, R., Jones, S., Pfaff, E., & Shinohara, M. (2013). Modeling data: A learning progression for supporting the development of statistical reasoning. Paper presented at the 2013 annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Building Learning Progressions for Science and Math Learning, San Francisco, CA: Vanderbilt University.

  • Limon, M. (2001). On the cognitive conflict as an instructional strategy for conceptual change: A Critical appraisal. Learning and Instruction, 11, 357–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linn, M., & Eylon, B. (2011). Science learning and instruction: Taking advantage of technology to promote knowledge integration. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L., & Boscolo, P. (2004). Role of epistemological understanding and interest in interpreting a controversy and in topic specific belief change. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 103–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L., & Gava, M. (2007). Effects of epistemological beliefs and learning text structure on conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou, A. Baltas, & X. Vamvakoussi (Eds.), Reframing the conceptual change approach in learning and instruction (pp. 165–196). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L., Gava, M., & Boldrin, A. (2008). On warm conceptual change: The interplay of text, epistemological beliefs, and topic interest. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 291–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ni, Y., & Zhou, Y.-D. (2005). Teaching and learning fraction and rational numbers: The origins and implications of whole number bias. Educational Psychologist, 40(1), 27–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum, J. (1979). Children’s conception of the earth as a cosmic body: A cross-age study. Science Education, 63, 83–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum, J., & Novak, J. D. (1976). An assessment of children’s concepts of the earth utilizing structured interviews. Science Education, 60, 535–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozdemir, G., & Clark, D. (2009). Knowledge structure coherence in Turkish students’ understanding of force. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46, 570–596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A., Hewson, P. W., & Gertzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: Towards a theory of conceptual change. Science Education, 66, 211–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiner, M., Slotta, J. D., Chi, M. T. H., & Resnick, L. B. (2000). Naïve physics reasoning: A commitment to substance-based conceptions. Cognition and Instruction, 18(1), 1–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sadler, D. R. (1987). Specifying and promulgating achievement standards. Oxford Review of Education, 13, 191–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samarapungavan, A., Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1996). Mental models of the earth, sun, and moon: Indian children’s cosmologies. Cognitive Development, 11, 491–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, C.V., Reiser, B. J., Davis, E. A., Kenyon, L., Acher, A., Fortus, D., Shwartz, Y., Hug, B., & Krajcik, J. (2009). Developing a learning progression for scientific modeling: Making scientific modeling accessible and meaningful for learners. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46(6), 632–654.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinatra, G. M., & Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.). (2003). Intentional conceptual change. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. P., diSessa, A. A., & Roschelle, J. (1993). Misconceptions reconceived: A constructivist analysis of knowledge in transition. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 3, 115–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C., Wiser, M., Anderson, C. W., & Krajcik, J. (2006). Implications for children’s learning for assessment: A proposed learning progression for matter and the atomic molecular theory. Measurement, 14(1&2), 1–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sneider, C., & Pulos, S. (1983). Children’s cosmographies: Understanding the earth’s shape and gravity. Science Education, 67, 205–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stafylidou, S., & Vosniadou, S. (2004). The development of students’ understanding of the numerical value of fractions. In L. Verschaffel, S. Vosniadou (Eds.), Conceptual change in mathematics learning and teaching, Special Issue of Learning and instruction 14, 5, 503–518.

  • Stathopoulou, C., & Vosniadou, S. (2007a). Conceptual change in physics and physics-related epistemological beliefs: A relationship under scrutiny. In S. Vosniadou, A. Baltas, & X. Vamvakoussi (Eds.), Reframing the conceptual change approach in learning and instruction (pp. 145–163). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stathopoulou, C., & Vosniadou, S. (2007b). Exploring the relationship between physics-related epistemological beliefs and physics understanding. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 255–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vamvakoussi, Χ., & Vosniadou, S. (2010). How many decimals are there between two fractions? Aspects of secondary school students’ reasoning about rational numbers and their notation. Cognition and Instruction, 28(2), 181–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Hoof, J., Lijnen, T., Verschaffel, L., & Van Dooren, W. (in press). Are secondary school students still hampered by the natural number bias? A Reaction time study on fraction comparison tasks. Research in Mathematics Education.

  • Vosniadou, S. (2003). Exploring the relationships between conceptual change and intentional learning. In G. M. Sinatra & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Intentional conceptual change (pp. 377–406). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S. (2013). Conceptual change in learning and instruction: The framework theory approach. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), The international handbook of conceptual change (2nd ed., pp. 11–30). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1994). Mental models of the day/night cycle. Cognitive Science, 18, 123–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., Ioannides, C., Dimitrakopoulou, A., & Papademetriou, E. (2001). Designing learning environments to promote conceptual change in science. Learning and Instruction, 11, 381–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Mason, L. (2011). Conceptual change induced by instruction: A complex interplay of multiple factors. In K. R. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook (Vol. 2, pp. 221–246). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Skopeliti, I. (2005). Developmental shifts in children’s categorization of the earth, In B. G. Bara, L. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the XXVII Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 2325–2330.

  • Vosniadou, S. & Skopeliti, I. (submitted). Children’s erroneous inferences in the comprehension of counter-intuitive science text. Learning and Instruction.

  • Vosniadou, S., Skopeliti, I., & Ikospentaki, K. (2004). Modes of knowing and ways of reasoning in elementary astronomy. Cognitive Development, 19, 203–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., Skopeliti, I., & Ikospentaki, K. (2005). Reconsidering the role of artifacts in reasoning: Children’s understanding of the globe as a model of the earth. Learning and Instruction, 15, 333–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., Vamvakoussi, X., & Skopeliti, I. (2008). The framework theory approach to the problem of conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 3–34). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Verschaffel, L. (2004). Extending the conceptual change approach to mathematics learning and teaching. In L. Verschaffel, & S. Vosniadou (Eds.), Conceptual change in mathematics learning and teaching. Special Issue of Learning and Instruction, 14(5), pp. 445–451.

  • Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development: Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 337–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiser, M., & Smith, C. L. (2008). Learning and teaching about matter in grades K-8: When should the atomic-molecular theory be introduced? In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), The international handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 205–239). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stella Vosniadou.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Vosniadou, S., Skopeliti, I. Conceptual Change from the Framework Theory Side of the Fence. Sci & Educ 23, 1427–1445 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-013-9640-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-013-9640-3

Keywords

Navigation