Skip to main content
Log in

The Relation of Story Structure to a Model of Conceptual Change in Science Learning

  • Published:
Science & Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although various reasons have been proposed to explain the potential effectiveness of science stories to promote learning, no explicit relationship of stories to learning theory in science has been propounded. In this paper, two structurally analogous models are developed and compared: a structural model of stories and a temporal conceptual change model of learning. On the basis of the similarity of the models, as elaborated, it is proposed that the structure of science stories may promote a re-enactment of the learning process, and, thereby, such stories serve to encourage active learning through the generation of hypotheses and explanations. The practical implications of this theoretical analogy can be applied to the classroom in that the utilization of stories provides the opportunity for a type of re-enactment of the learning process that may encourage both engagement with the material and the development of long-term memory structures.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arntzenius, F. (1995). A heuristic for conceptual change. Philosophy of Science, 62, 357–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bremond, C. (1980). The logic of narrative possibilities. New Literary History, 11, 387–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1996). Science education as conceptual change. Paper presented for the Committee on developments in the science of learning for the sciences of science learning: An interdisciplinary discussion, New York University, Department of Psychology.

  • Carey, S., Evans, R., Honda, M., Jay, E., & Ungar, C. (1990). An experiment is when you try and see if it works. International Journal of Science Education, 11, 514–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duit, R., & Treagust, D. F. (1998). Learning in science—from behaviourism towards social constructivism and beyond. In B. J. Fraser & K. G. Tobin (Eds.), International handbook of science education (pp. 3–25). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, K. (1978). What is a plot? New Literary History, 9, 455–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egan, K. (1986). Teaching as story telling. London, Ontario: Althouse Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, K. (1989a). The shape of the science text: A function of stories. In S. de Castell, A. Luke, & C. Luke (Eds.), Language, authority and criticism: Readings on the school textbook (pp. 96–108). New York: The Falmer Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, K. (1989b). Memory, imagination, and learning: Connected by the story. Phi Delta Kappan, 70(6), 455–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., Glymour, C., Sobel, D. M., Schultz, L. E., Kushnir, T., & Hanks, D. (2004). A theory of causal learning in children: Causal maps and bayes nets. Psychological Review, 111(1), 3–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A., & Weimer-Hastings, K. (1999). Situation models and concepts in story comprehension. In S. Goldman, A. Graesser, & P. van den Broek (Eds.), Narrative comprehension, causality and coherence (pp. 77–92). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harman, G. (1965). The inference to the best explanation. Philosophical Review, 74, 421–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harnad, S. (1982). Neoconstructivism: A unifying constraint for the cognitive sciences. In T. Simon & R. Scholes (Eds.), Language, mind and brain (pp. 1–11). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helstrand, A., & Ott, A. (1995). The utilization of fiction when teaching the theory of relativity. Physics Education, 30(5), 284–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewson, P. W. (1981). A conceptual change approach to learning science. European Journal of Science Education, 3(4), 383–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, P. J. (2000). The owner’s manual for the brain: Everyday applications from mind-brain research (2nd ed.). Austin: Bard Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Josephson, J. R., & Josephson, S. G. (1994). Abductive inference, computation philosophy, technology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenealy, P. (1989). Telling a coherent “Story”: A role for the history and philosophy of science in a physical science course. In D. E. Herget (Ed.), HPSST, Proceedings of the First International Conference, pp 209–220.

  • Klassen, S. (2009). The construction and analysis of a science story: A proposed methodology. Science & Education, 18, 401–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruckeberg, R. (2006). A Deweyan perspective on science education: Constructivism, experience, and why we learn science. Science & Education, 15, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kubli, F. (1999). Historical aspects in physics teaching: Using Galileo’s work in a New Swiss project. Science & Education, 8, 137–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levi-Strauss, C. (1966). The savage mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, H. (1998). The effectiveness of teaching chemistry through the history of science. J Chemical Education, 75(10), 1326–1330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, D. (1992). Science as writing. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macbeth, D. (2000). On an actual apparatus for conceptual change. Science Education, 84, 228–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magie, W. F. (1965). A source book in physics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, N. S. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, W. (1986). Recent theories of narrative. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, B. E., & Brouwer, W. (1991). The sharing of personal science and the narrative element in science education. Science Education, 75(6), 707–722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1984). What makes a good story? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 13(6), 457–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metz, D., Klassen, S., McMillan, B., Clough, M., & Olson, J. (2007). Building a foundation for historical narratives. Science & Education, 16, 313–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miall, D. S., & Kuiken, D. (1994). Foregrounding, defamiliarization, and affect response to literary stories. Poetics, 22, 389–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noddings, N., & Witherell, C. (1991). Epilogue: Themes remembered and foreseen. In C. Witherell & N. Noddings (Eds.), Stories lives tell (pp. 279–280). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, S., Guilbert, M., Smith, M., Shahram, H., & Phillips, L. (2005). A theoretical framework for narrative explanation in science. Science Education, 89(4), 535–554.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohlsson, S. (1992). The cognitive skill of theory articulation: A neglected aspect of science education? Science & Education, 1, 181–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohlsson, S. (1999). Theoretical commitment and implicit knowledge: Why anomalies do not trigger learning. Science & Education, 8, 559–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohlsson, S. (2002). Generating and understanding qualitative explanations. In A. Graesser, J. Leon, & J. Otero (Eds.), The psychology of science text comprehension (pp. 91–128). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A., Hewson, P. W., & Gertzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change. Science Education, 66(2), 211–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prince, G. (1973). A grammar of stories: An introduction. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reid, I. (1977). The short story. London: Methuen & Co Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romero, F., Paris, S. G., & Brem, S. (2005). Children’s comprehension and local-to-global recall of narrative and expository texts. Current Issues in Education 8(25): Available via http://cie.ed.asu.edu/volume8/number25/. Accessed 1 July 2009.

  • Schwitzgebel, E. (1999). Children’s theories and the drive to explain. Science & Education, 8, 457–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, J. (2002). Science stories and science texts: What can they do for our students? Studies in Science Education, 37, 85–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, J., Duveen, J., Scot, L., & McCarthy, S. (1992). Teaching about the nature of science through history: Action research in the classroom. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 29(4), 409–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stinner, A. (1990). Philosophy, thought experiments and large context problems in the secondary school physics course. International Journal of Science Education, 12(3), 244–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stinner, A. (1992). Contextual teaching in physics: From science stories to large—context problems. Alberta Journal of Science Education, 26(1), 20–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strike, K. A., & Posner, G. J. (1982). Conceptual change and science teaching. European Journal of Science Education, 4(3), 231–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, R. S. (1997). On the significance of Markov decision processes. In W. Gerstner, A. Germond, M. Hasler, & J. D. Nicoud (Eds.), Artificial neural networks—ICANN’97 (pp. 273–282). London: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndyke, P. W. (1977). Cognitive structures in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 77–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wandersee, J. H. (1990). On the value and use of the history of science in teaching today’s science: Constructing historical vignettes. In D. E. Herget (Ed.), More history and philosophy of science in science teaching (pp. 278–283). Tallahassee, FL: Florida State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wandersee, J. H. (1992). The historicality of cognition: Implications for science education research. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 29(4), 423–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wandersee, J. H., & Roach, L. M. (1998). Interactive historical vignettes. In J. J. Mintzes, J. H. Wandersee, & J. D. Novak (Eds.), Teaching science for understanding (pp. 281–306). California: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, A. N. (1929). The aims of education and other essays. New York: McMillan.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The author thanks Arthur Stinner for insightful discussions that contributed to the writing of this paper. Funding provided by the University of Winnipeg and Canada’s NSERC CRYSTAL centre at the University of Manitoba has supported the researching and writing of this paper. Appreciation is also due to Carly Scramstad for her invaluable assistance with the final revision of the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephen Klassen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Klassen, S. The Relation of Story Structure to a Model of Conceptual Change in Science Learning. Sci & Educ 19, 305–317 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-009-9212-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-009-9212-8

Keywords

Navigation