Skip to main content
Log in

The Application of Historical Narrative in Science Learning: The Atlantic Cable Story

  • Published:
Science & Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The use of historically based stories to teach science has both theoretical and practical support. This paper outlines how the historically based story may be utilized effectively in the classroom and, as an illustration of this, presents the story of Lord Kelvin’s role in the laying of the first trans-Atlantic communications cable during the period from 1857 to 1866. Expected and observed classroom benefits that accrue from this approach are summarized. The paper concludes with an outline of a program of research which incorporates the development of historically based stories.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Blake-Coleman B.C. (1992). Copper Wire and Electrical Conductors: The Shaping of a Technology. Harwood Academic Publishers, Chur, Switzerland

    Google Scholar 

  • Bachelard, G.: 1938, La Formation de l'Esprit Scientifique, Vrin, Paris

  • Bright C. (1898). Submarine Telegraphs: Their History, Construction and Working. Corsby Lockwood, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Bright C. (1903). The Story of the Atlantic Cable. George Newnes, Ltd., London

    Google Scholar 

  • Cox M.T., Ram A. (1999). On the intersection of story understanding and learning. In: Ram A., Moorman K. (eds), Understanding Language Understanding: Computational Models of Reading. MIT Press/Bradford Books, Cambridge, MA, pp. 391–434

    Google Scholar 

  • Dibner B. (1959). The Atlantic Cable. Burndy Library, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan K. (1986). Teaching as Story Telling. Althouse Press, London, Ontario

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, K.: 1989, ‚Memory, Imagination, and Learning: Connected by the Story’, Phi Delta Kappan 70(6): 455–459.

  • Faraday M. (1854/1965). On Electric Induction – Associated Cases of Current and Static Effects. In Faraday M. (ed.), Experimental Researches in Electricity, Vol. III. Dover Publications, Inc., New York, pp. 508–520

    Google Scholar 

  • Gil-Pérez D. (2002). Defending Constructivism in Science Education. Science & Education 11:557–571

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helstrand A., Ott A. (1995). The Utilization of Fiction when Teaching the Theory of Relativity. Physics Education 30(5):284–286

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenealy, P.: 1989, ‚Telling a Coherent “Story”: A Role for the History and Philosophy of Science in a Physical Science Course’, in D.E. Herget (ed.), HPSST, Proc. of the First Int. Conference, pp. 209–220.

  • Kimmel & Foster, pubs.: 1866, The Eighth Wonder of the World: The Atlantic Cable, Hand coloured lithograph, Library of Congress, Prints and Photographs Division, Kimmel and Foster, New York. [Image available on-line] http://www.lcweb.loc.gov/exhibits/british/brit-5.html.

  • Klassen, S.: in press, ‚A Theoretical Framework for Contextual Science Teaching’, Interchange.

  • Kubli F. (2005). Science Teaching as a Dialogue – Bakhtin, Vygotsky and some Applications in the Classroom. Science & Education 14(6):501–534

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, B.E., Brouwer W. (1991). The Sharing of Personal Science and the Narrative Element in Science Education. Science & Education 75(6):707–722

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews M. (1994). Science Teaching: The Role of History and Philosophy of Science. Routledge, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, S. & Brew, C.: 2001, ‚A Rational Analysis of Semantic Processing by the Left Cerebral Hemisphere’, First Workshop on Cognitively Plausible Models of Semantic Processing (SEMPRO-2001), Edinburgh. July 31, 2001.

  • Metz, D., Klassen, S., McMillan, B., Clough, M. & Olson, J.: in press, ‚Building a Foundation for the Use of Historical Narratives, Science & Education.

  • Miall D.S., Kuiken D. (1994). Foregrounding, Defamiliarization, and Affect Response to Literary Stories. Poetics 22:389–407

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michael E.B., Keller T.A., Carpenter P.A., Just M.A. (2001). fMRI Investigation of Sentence Comprehension by Eye and by Ear: Modality Fingerprints on Cognitive Processes. Human Brain Mapping 13:239–252

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monk M., Osborne J.(1997). Placing the History and Philosophy of Science on the Curriculum: A Model for the Development of Pedagogy. Science & Education 81:405–424

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norris S.P., Guilbert S.M., Smith M.L., Hakimelahi S., Philips L.M. (2005). A Theoretical Framework for Narrative Explanation in Science. Science & Education 89(4):535–563

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth W., Roychoudhury A. (1993). The Development of Science Process Skills in Authentic Contexts. Journal of Research in Science Teaching 30:127–152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stinner A., McMillan B., Metz D., Jilek J., Klassen S. (2003). The Renewal of Case Studies in Science Education. Science & Education 12(7):617–643

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson S.P. (1910). The Life of William Thomson, Baron Kelvin of Largs. Macmillan, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomson W. (1855/1884a). On the Theory of the Electric Telegraph. In: Thomson W. (ed.), Mathematical and Physical Papers, Vol. II. The University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 61–75

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomson W. (1855/1884b). On Peristaltic Induction of Electric Currents in Submarine Telegraph Wires. In: Thomson W. (ed.), Mathematical and Physical Papers, Vol. II. The University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 77–91

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomson W. (1860/1884). Analytical and Synthetical Attempts to Ascertain the Cause of the Differences of Electric Conductivity in Wires of Nearly Pure Copper. In: Thomson W. (ed.), Mathematical and Physical Papers, Vol. II. The University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 118–128

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomson W. (1865/1884). On the Forces Concerned in the Laying and Lifting of Deep-Sea Cables. In: Thomson W. (ed.), Mathematical and Physical Papers, Vol. II. The University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 153–167

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomson W. (1889/1890). Ether, Electricity, and Ponderable Matter. In: Thomson W. (ed), Mathematical and Physical Papers, Vol. III: Elasticity, Heat, Electro-magnetism. Cambridge University Press Warehouse, London, pp. 484–515

    Google Scholar 

  • Vipond D., Hunt R.A. (1984). Point-Driven Understanding: Pragmatic and Cognitive Dimensions of Literary Reading. Poetics 13, 261–277

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The researching and writing of this paper was made possible, in part, through funding provided by The University of Winnipeg and the NSERC CRYSTAL at The University of Manitoba.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephen Klassen.

Appendices

Appendix: A Literary Story

The Galvanometer

The telegraphic cable-laying ships, the Agamemnon, and the Niagara, finally met in mid-Atlantic on July 29, 1858. Professor Thomson was stationed aboard the Agamemnon. It is rather an exciting occupation to watch the tell-tale signals on the Professor’s galvanometer as the cable pays out. Indeed, it is almost impossible to realize the anxiety and heart-interest everybody manifests in the undertaking. Few, but the crew, even sleep soundly. Professor Thomson frequently does not put off his clothes at night.

Tonight, but a few hours after starting, there was an alarming crisis. The Agamemnon had signalled to the Niagara, ‚Forty miles submerged,’ and she was just beginning her acknowledgment, when suddenly, at 10 PM, communication ceased. According to orders, those on duty sent at once for Dr. Thomson. He came in a fearful state of excitement. One of the crew overheard him muttering to himself as he came: ‚I shall have to use the bridge arrangement of Professor Wheatstone’. He supposed the fault might lie in a suspicious portion, which had been observed in the main coil, as, indeed, the tests confirmed. Not a second was to be lost, for it was evident that the damaged portion must be payed overboard in a few minutes; and, in the meantime, the tedious and difficult operation of making a splice had to be performed. Nearly all the officers of the ship and of those connected with the expedition stood in groups about the coil, watching with intense anxiety the cable as it slowly unwound itself nearer and nearer the joint, while the electricians worked at the splice as only persons could work who felt that the life and death of the expedition depended upon their rapidity. When the splice was finished, the signal was made to loose the brakes, and the repaired section of cable passed overboard in safety.

Attention now turned to the electrical room where the scene was such as those present shall never forget. The two clerks on duty, watching, with the common anxiety depicted on their faces, for a propitious signal; Dr. Thomson, in a perfect fever of nervous excitement, shaking like an aspen leaf, yet in mind clear and collected, testing and waiting, with half-despairing look for the result. Behind, in the darker part of the room, stood various officers of the ship. Round the door crowded the sailors of the watch, peeping over each other’s shoulders at the mysteries, and shouting ‚gangway!’ when any one of importance wished to enter. The eyes of all were directed to the instruments, watching for the slightest quiver indicative of life. Such a scene was never witnessed save by the bedside of the dying. Things continued thus. After some minutes, Dr. Thomson and the others left the room, convinced they were doomed to disappointment. Suddenly one sang out, ‚Haloa! the spot has gone up to 40°’. The clerk at the measuring instrument bolted right out of the room, scarcely knowing where he went for joy; ran to the deck, and cried out, ‚Mr. Thomson! the cable’s all right; we got a signal from the Niagara’. When the first stun of surprise and pleasure passed, each one began trying to express his feelings in some way more or less energetic. Dr. Thomson laughed right loud and heartily. Never was more anxiety compressed into such a space of time and never was there more relief. The entire incident of signal failure lasted exactly one hour and a half, but it did not seem a third of that time. Afterward, we learned that a faulty sand battery aboard the Niagara had prevented them from responding to our signals immediately. (Adapted from Thompson 1910, pp. 361–363 and Bright 1903, pp. 119–121)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Klassen, S. The Application of Historical Narrative in Science Learning: The Atlantic Cable Story. Sci Educ 16, 335–352 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-006-9026-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-006-9026-x

Keywords

Navigation