Araújo, S., Reis, A., Petersson, K. M., & Faísca, L. (2015). Rapid automatized naming and reading performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 868–883. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000006
Article
Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(2), 12–28.
Article
Google Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Article
Google Scholar
Ben-Dror, I., Bentin, S., & Frost, R. (1995). Semantic, phonologic, and morphologic skills in reading disabled and normal children: Evidence from perception and production of spoken Hebrew. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 876. https://doi.org/10.2307/748202
Article
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K.F. & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525–531. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990423.
Bowers, P. (1995). Tracing symbol naming speed’s unique contributions to reading disabilities over time. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 189–216. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf01027185
Article
Google Scholar
Bowers, P., Golden, J., Kennedy, A., & Young, A. (1994). Limits upon orthographic knowledge due to processes indexed by naming speed. Neuropsychology and Cognition, 8, 173–218. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-3492-9_6
Article
Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (2005). Predicting individual differences in learning to read. In M. J. Snowling, & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 155–172). Oxford: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470757642.ch9.
Bradbury, B., Corak, M., Waldfogel, J., & Washbrook, E. (2015). Too many children left behind: The U.S. achievement gap in comparative perspective. Russell Sage Foundation.
Brysbaert, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E. (2018). The word frequency effect in word processing: An updated review. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 45–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417727521
Article
Google Scholar
Burani, C., Marcolini, S., & Stella, G. (2002). How early does morpholexical reading develop in readers of a shallow orthography? Brain and Language, 81, 568–586. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2001.2548
Article
Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Mousikou, P., Efrim, C., Litavský, M., Onochie-Quintanilla, E., Salas, N., Schöffelová, M., Defior, S., Mikulajová, M., Seidlová-Málková, G., & Hulme, C. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science, 23, 678–686. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611434536
Article
Google Scholar
Castles, A., Rastle, K., & Nation, K. (2018). Ending the reading wars: Reading acquisition from novice to expert. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 19(1), 5–51. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100618772271.
Article
Google Scholar
Chang, Y.-N., & Monaghan, P. (2018). Quantity and diversity of preliteracy language exposure both affect literacy development: Evidence from a computational model of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23, 235–253. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1529177
Article
Google Scholar
Chang, Y.-N., Monaghan, P., & Welbourne, S. (2019). A computational model of reading across development: Effects of literacy onset on language processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 108, 104025. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2019.05.003
Article
Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.108.1.204
Article
Google Scholar
De Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2003). Developmental changes in the manifestation of a phonological deficit in dyslexic children learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 22–40. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.22
Article
Google Scholar
de Jong, P. F., & van der Leji, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–476. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.450
Article
Google Scholar
Ennemoser, M., Marx, P., Weber, J., & Schneider, W. (2012). Spezifische Vorläuferfertigkeiten der Lesegeschwindigkeit, des Leseverständnisses und des Rechtschreibens. (Specific precursor skills of reading speed, reading comprehension and writing). Zeitschrift Für Entwicklungspsychologie Und Pädagogische Psychologie, 44, 53–67. https://doi.org/10.1026/0049-8637/a000057
Article
Google Scholar
Glaser, W. R. (1992). Picture naming. Cognition, 42, 61–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90040-o
Article
Google Scholar
Grainger, J., Lété, B., Bertand, D., Dufau, S., & Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Evidence for multiple routes in learning to read. Cognition, 123(2), 280–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.01.003.
Article
Google Scholar
Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111(3), 662–720. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.111.3.662.
Article
Google Scholar
Hasenäcker, J., Verra, L., & Schroeder, S. (2019). Comparing length and frequency effects in children across modalities. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 1682–1691. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021818805063
Article
Google Scholar
Hothorn, T., Bretz, F., & Westfall, P. (2008). Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biometrical Journal, 50(3), 346–363. https://doi.org/10.1002/bimj.200810425.
Article
Google Scholar
Huestegge, L., Radach, R., Corbic, D., & Huestegge, S. M. (2009). Oculomotor and linguistic determinants of reading development: A longitudinal study. Vision Research, 49, 2948–2959. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.09.012
Article
Google Scholar
Hyönä, J., & Olson, R. K. (1995). Eye fixation patterns among dyslexic and normal readers: Effects of word length and word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1430–1440. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.6.1430
Article
Google Scholar
Jackson, N. E., & Coltheart, M. (2001). Routes to reading success and failure: Toward an integrated cognitive psychology of atypical reading. Taylor & Francis.
Google Scholar
Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). Wuggy: A multilingual pseudoword generator. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 627–633. https://doi.org/10.3758/brm.42.3.627
Article
Google Scholar
Landerl, K., Freudenthaler, H. H., Heene, M., de Jong, P. F., Desroches, A., Manolitsis, G., & Parrila, R. (2018). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming as longitudinal predictors of reading in five alphabetic orthographies with varying degrees of consistency. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23, 220–234. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1510936
Article
Google Scholar
Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150–161. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.150
Article
Google Scholar
Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315–334. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(97)00005-x
Article
Google Scholar
Martens, V. E. G., & de Jong, P. F. (2006). The effect of word length on lexical decision in dyslexic and normal reading children. Brain and Language, 98, 140–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.003
Article
Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S.-A.H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322–352. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026744ƒ
Article
Google Scholar
Moll, K., Fussenegger, B., Willburger, E., & Landerl, K. (2009). RAN is not a measure of orthographic processing. Evidence from the asymmetric German orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430802631684
Article
Google Scholar
Moll, K., & Landerl, K. (2010). SLRT-II: Lese- und Rechtschreibtest: Weiterentwicklung der Salzburger Lese- und Rechtschreibtests (SLRT): Manual [SLRT-II: Reading and writing test: Advancement of the Salzburg reading and writing test (SLRT)]. Verlag Hans Huber.
Google Scholar
Moll, K., Ramus, F., Bartling, J., Bruder, J., Kunze, S., Neuhoff, N., & Landerl, K. (2014). Cognitive mechanisms underlying reading and spelling development in five European orthographies. Learning and Instruction, 29, 65–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.09.003
Article
Google Scholar
Ouellette, G. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 554–566. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.554
Article
Google Scholar
Pauly, H., Linkersdörfer, J., Lindberg, S., Woerner, W., Hasselhorn, M., & Lonnemann, J. (2011). Domain-specific rapid automatized naming deficits in children at risk for learning disabilities. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24, 602–610. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.02.002
Article
Google Scholar
Perry, C., Zorzi, M., & Ziegler, J. C. (2019). Understanding dyslexia through personalized large-scale computational models. Psychological Science, 30, 386–395. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618823540
Article
Google Scholar
Pritchard, S. C., Coltheart, M., Marinus, E., & Castles, A. (2018). A computational model of the self-teaching hypothesis based on the Dual-Route Cascaded model of reading. Cognitive Science, 42, 722–770. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12571
Article
Google Scholar
R Development Core Team (2008). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0. http://www.R-project.org.
Raiche, G. (2010). An R package for parallel analysis and non graphical solutions to the Cattell scree test. R package version 2.3.3.1. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nFactors.
Rau, A. K., Moeller, K., & Landerl, K. (2014). The transition from sublexical to lexical processing in a consistent orthography: An eye-tracking study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 224–233. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.857673
Article
Google Scholar
Rau, A. K., Moll, K., Moeller, K., Huber, S., Snowling, M. J., & Landerl, K. (2016). Same same, but different: Word and sentence reading in German and English. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(3), 203–219. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2015.1136913
Article
Google Scholar
Reicher, G. M. (1969). Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulus material. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 275–280. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027768
Article
Google Scholar
Rueckl, J. G. (2016). Toward a theory of variation in the organization of the word reading system. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20, 86–97. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2015.1103741
Article
Google Scholar
Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., Coltheart, M., & Castles, A. (2015). Getting to the bottom of orthographic depth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1614–1629. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0835
Article
Google Scholar
Schroeder, S., Würzner, K.-M., Heister, J., Geyken, A., & Kliegl, R. (2014). childLex: A lexical database of German read by children. Behavior Research Methods, 47, 1085–1094. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0528-1
Article
Google Scholar
Schröter, P., & Schroeder, S. (2017). The Developmental Lexicon Project: A behavioral database to investigate visual word recognition across the lifespan. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0851-9
Article
Google Scholar
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2
Article
Google Scholar
Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 95–129. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1998.2481
Article
Google Scholar
Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584
Article
Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (Eds.). (1994). Reading development and dyslexia. London, UK: Whurr.
Spinelli, D., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Mancini, M., Martelli, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2005). Length effect in word naming in reading: Role of reading experience and reading deficit in Italian readers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 217–235. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2702_2
Article
Google Scholar
Stock, C., Marx, P., & Schneider, W. (2003): BAKO 1–4. Basiskompetenzen für Lese-Rechtschreibleistungen. Ein Test zur Erfassung der phonologischen Bewusstheit vom ersten bis vierten Grundschuljahr. (BAKO 1–4. Basic competencies for reading and writing abilites. A test of phonological awareness from grade one through four.) Göttingen: Beltz Test GmbH.
Swan, D., & Goswami, U. (1997). Picture naming deficits in developmental dyslexia: The phonological representations hypothesis. Brain and Language, 56, 334–353. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1997.1855
Article
Google Scholar
Szekely, A., Jacobsen, T., D’Amico, S., Devescovi, A., Andonova, E., Herron, D., Lug, C. C., Pechmann, T., Pléh, C., Wicha, N., Federmeier, K., Gerdjikova, I., Gutierrez, G., Hung, D., Hsug, J., Iyer, G., Kohnert, K., Mehotcheva, T., Orozco-Figueroa, A., et al. (2004). A new on-line resource for psycholinguistic studies. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 247–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.03.002
Article
Google Scholar
Tiffin-Richards, S. P., & Schroeder, S. (2015). Word length and frequency effects on children’s eye movements during silent reading. Vision Research, 113, 33–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2015.05.008
Article
Google Scholar
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second- to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161–185. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0102_4
Article
Google Scholar
Van den Boer, M., & de Jong, P. F. (2015). Parallel and serial reading processes in children’s word and nonword reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 141–151. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037101.
Article
Google Scholar
Van den Boer, M., de Jong, P. F., & Haentjens-van Meeteren, M. M. (2012). Lexical decision in children: Sublexical processing or lexical search? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1214–1228. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.652136
Article
Google Scholar
Van den Boer, M., de Jong, P. F., & Haentjens-van Meeteren, M. M. (2013). Modeling the length effect: Specifying the relation with visual and phonological correlates of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(4), 243–256. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2012.683222.
Article
Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x
Article
Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.192
Article
Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., Saine, N., Lyytinen, H., Vaessen, A., & Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading. Psychological Science, 21(4), 551–559. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406
Article
Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Castel, C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., Alario, F.-X., & Perry, C. (2008). Developmental dyslexia and the dual route model of reading: Simulating individual differences and subtypes. Cognition, 107, 151–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.004
Article
Google Scholar
Ziegler, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1995). Phonological information provides early sources of constraint in the processing of letter strings. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 567–593. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1026
Article
Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Coltheart, M. (2000). The DRC model of visual word recognition and reading aloud: An extension to German. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 413–430. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440050114570
Article
Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169–193. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00139-5
Article
Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Modelling reading development through phonological decoding and self-teaching: Implications for dyslexia. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20120397. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0397
Article
Google Scholar
Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Gasperini, F., Judica, A., & Spinelli, D. (2005). Word length effect in early reading and in developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 93, 369–373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.010
Article
Google Scholar