Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

How do latent decoding and language predict latent reading comprehension: across two years in grades 5, 7, and 9?

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This investigation examined the structural relations among latent variables of language, decoding, and reading comprehension, invariance of the patterns of predictions, and unique and common variance over two years in grade cohorts 5 to 6, 7 to 8, and 9 to 10. Participants were 321 students in grade 5 in six elementary schools, 299 students in grade 7 in three middle schools, and 137 students in grades 9 in one high school in Florida. The dimensionality of language measures (vocabulary and syntax) and decoding measures (real word and nonword fluency) was examined using confirmatory factor analysis and related to a reading comprehension factor consisting of state and national tests. Structural relations among language, decoding, and reading comprehension were stable across grade cohorts. The interaction of decoding and language in predicting reading comprehension was significant only in the grade 5 cohort, thereby confirming a multiplicative functional form only in that cohort. Multiple group invariance testing using structural equation modeling allowed results across grade cohorts to be compared, revealing the increasing importance of language over decoding to predicting reading comprehension in the middle and high school cohorts. By high school, language and reading comprehension were essentially one dimension. The contribution of shared variance in language comprehension and decoding in jointly predicting reading comprehension was large in all cohorts. Implications for the Simple View of Reading and for education are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ahmed, Y., Francis, D., York, M., Fletcher, J., Barnes, M., & Kulesz, P. (2016). Validation of the direct and inferential mediation (DIME) model of reading comprehension in grades 7 through 12. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 44–45, 68–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.02.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Best, R., Floyd, R., & McNamara, D. (2008). Differential competencies contributing to children’s comprehension of narrative and expository texts. Reading Psychology, 29, 137–164. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702710801963951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M., Hahn, C.-S., Putnick, D., & Suwalsky, J. (2014). Stability of core language skill from early childhood to adolescence: A latent variable approach. Child Development, 85(4), 1346–1356. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Browne, M., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 31–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (2008). Comprehensive assessment of spoken language. Torrance. CA: Western Psychological Services.

  • Castles, A., Rastle, K., & Nation, K. (2018). Ending the reading wars: Reading acquisition from novice to expert. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 19(1), 5–51. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100618772271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H., Adlof, S., & Weismer, S. (2006). Language deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the Simple View of Reading. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 278–293. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/023).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H., Herrera, S., Nielsen, D., & Bridges, (2015). Early prediction of reading comprehension within the simple view framework. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 28, 1407–1425. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9576-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, R. S., & Vellutino, F. (1997). Prediction of reading ability: A cross-validation study of the simple view of reading. Journal of Literacy Research, 29(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/10862969709547947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, P., Snowling, M., Truelove, E., & Hulme, C. (2010). Ameliorating children’s reading-comprehension difficulties: A randomized controlled trial. Psychological Science, 21(8), 1106–1116. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610375449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cromley, J., & Azevedo, R. (2007). Testing and refining the direct and inferential mediation model of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 311–325. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, L., & Scarborough, H. (2006). Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 277–299. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr1003_5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyer, L., & Katz, L. (1992). An examination of “The Simple View of Reading.” Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research. SR-111/112, 161–166. Retrieved April 7, 2020 from http://www.haskins.yale.edu/sr/sr111/sr111_12.pdf.

  • Dunn, L., & Dunn, D. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test-4. San Antonio: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eason, S., Sabatini, J., Goldberg, L., Bruce, K., & Cutting, L. (2013). Examining the relationship between word reading efficiency and oral reading rate in predicting comprehension among different types of readers. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(3), 199–223. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2011.652722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Educational Testing Service. (2005). Comprehensive english language learning assessment CELLA: Technical Report. Princeton, NJ: Author.

  • Finney, S., & DiStefano, C. (2006). Non-normal and categorical data in structural equation modeling. In G. Hancock & R. Mueller (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: A second course (pp. 269–314). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Florida Department of Education. (2013). Florida statewide assessments 2013 yearbook. Tallahassee, FL: Author.

  • Foorman, B., Beyler, N., Borradaile, K., Coyne, M., Denton, C., Dimino, J., Furgeson, J., Hayes, L., Henke, J., Justice, L., Keating, B., Lewis, W., Sattar, S., Streke, A., Wagner, R., & Wissel, S. (2016). Foundational skills to support reading for understanding in kindergarten through 3rd grade (NCEE 2016-4008). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance (NCEE), Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved April 7, 2020 from https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/Docs/PracticeGuide/wwc_foundationalreading_040717.pdf.

  • Foorman, B., Koon, S., Petscher, Y., Mitchell, A., & Truckenmiller, A. (2015). Examining General and Specific Factors in the Dimensionality of Oral Language and Reading in 4th-10th Grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 884–899. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B., Petscher, Y., & Herrera, S. (2018). Unique and common effects of decoding and language factors in predicting reading comprehension in grades 1–10. Learning and Individual Differences, 63, 12–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2018.02.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francis, D., Kulesz, P., & Benoit, J. (2018). Extending the simple view of reading to account for variation within readers and across texts: The Complete View of Reading (CVRi). Remedial and Special Education, 39(5), 274–288. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932518772904.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francis, D., Snow, C., August, D., Carlson, C., Miller, J., & Iglesias, A. (2006). Measures of reading comprehension: A latent variable analysis of the diagnostic assessment of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 301–322. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr1003_6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • García, J. R., & Cain, K. (2014). Decoding and reading comprehension: A meta-analysis to identify which reader and assessment characteristics influence the strength of the relationship in English. Review of Educational Research, 84(1), 74–111. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654313499616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P., & Tunmer, W. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and special Education, 7, 6–10. https://doi.org/10.1177/074193258600700104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J., Taylor, B., Olchowski, A., & Cumsille, P. (2006). Planned missing data designs in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 11(4), 323–343. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.11.4.323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammill, D., & Wiederholt, J. L. (2006). Test of silent contextual reading fluency. Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, H. D., Dunbar, S. B., & Frisbie, D. A. (2001). Iowa tests of basic skills (ITBS) forms A, B, and C. Rolling Meadows: Riverside Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W., & Gough, P. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127–160. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00401799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W., & Tunmer, W. (2018). The simple view of reading: Three assessments of its adequacy. Remedial and Special Education, 39(5), 304–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kershaw, S., & Schatschneider, C. (2012). A latent variable approach to the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 25, 433–464. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9278-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kieffer, M. J., Petscher, Y., Proctor, P., & Silverman, R. D. (2016). Is the whole greater than the sum of its parts? Modeling the contributions of language comprehension skills to reading comprehension in the upper elementary grades. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20, 436–454. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2016.1214591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., Hemphill, L., Troyer, M., Thomson, J., Jones, S., LaRusso, M., et al. (2017). Engaging struggling adolescent readers to improve reading skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 52(3), 357–382. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Language and Reading Research Consortium. (2015). Learning to read: Should we keep things simple? Reading Research Quarterly, 50(2), 151–169. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Language and Reading Research Consortium, & Chiu, Y. D. (2018). The simple view of reading across development: Prediction of grade 3 reading comprehension from prekindergarten skills. Remedial and Special Education, 39(5), 289–303. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932518762055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepola, J., Lynch, J., Kiuru, N., Laakkonen, E., & Niemi, P. (2016). Early oral language comprehension, task orientation, and foundational reading skills as predictors of grade 3 reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 51, 373–390. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83, 1198–1202. https://doi.org/10.2307/2290157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C., Burgess, S., & Schatschneider, C. (2018). Examining the simple view of reading with elementary school children: Still simple after all these years. Remedial and Special Education, 39(5), 260–273. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932518764833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1(3), 130–149. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacGinitie, W., MacGinitie, R., Maria, K., & Dreyer, L. (2000). Gates-MacGinitie reading tests (4th ed.). Rolling Meadows: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mancilla-Martinez, J., & Lesaux, N. (2010). Predictors of reading comprehension for struggling readers: The case of Spanish-speaking language minority children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 701–711. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meredith, W. (1993). Measurement invariance, factor analysis and factorial invariance. Psychometrika, 58(4), 525–543. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02294825.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morsy, L., Kieffer, M., & Snow, C. (2010). Measure for Measure: A critical consumers' guide to reading comprehension assessments for adolescents. Final Report from Carnegie Corporation of New York's Council on Advancing Adolescent Literacy. Carnegie Corporation of New York. Retrieved April 7, 2020 from https://media.carnegie.org/filer_public/32/11/3211dffc-7986-42fb-aee1-8d379664f3c6/ccny_report_2010_tta_morsy.pdf.

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 665–681. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (1998–2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author.

  • Neuhaus, G., Roldan, L., Boulware-Gooden, R., & Swank, P. (2006). Parsimonious reading models: Identifying teachable subskills. Reading Psychology, 27, 37–58. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702710500468724.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, M., Swank, P., Taylor, H., Landry, S., & Barnes, M. (2013). Effect of preschool working memory, language, and narrative abilities on inferential comprehension at school-age in children with spina bifida myelomeningocele and typically developing children. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 390–399. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617712001579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Mouzaki, A., Sideridis, G., Kotsolakou, R., & Simos, P. (2013). The role of vocabulary in the context of the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 29, 168–202. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2013.758569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, J. M., & Wagner, R. K. (2018). Using meta-analytic structural equation modeling to study developmental change in relations between language and literacy. Child Development, 89(6), 1956–1969. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wigg, E., & Secord, W. (2003). The clinical evaluation of language fundamentals, 4th edition: Examiner’s manual. San Antonio: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D., & Welsh, C. (1973). Elicited imitation as a research tool in developmental psycholinguistics. In C. Ferguson & D. Slobin (Eds.), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. (2018). Simple and not-so-simple views of reading. Remedial and Special Education, 39(5), 313–316. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932518770288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storch, S., & Whitehurst, G. R. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal, structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934–947. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tighe, E., & Schatschneider, C. (2014). A dominance analysis approach to determining predictor importance in third, seventh, and tenth grade reading comprehension skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 101–127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9435-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L., & wan Leeuwe, J. (2008). Prediction of the development of reading comprehension: A longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 407–423. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J., & Rashotte, C. (2012). Test of word reading efficiency (2nd ed.). Austin: ProEd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wanzek, J., Wexler, J., Vaughn, S., & Ciullo, S. (2010). Reading interventions for struggling readers in the upper elementary grades: a synthesis of 20 years of research. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 889–912. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9179-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiederholt, J. L., & Bryant, B. R. (2001). Gray oral reading tests-fourth edition. Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive vocabulary test–second edition (EVT-2). Minneapolis: Pearson Assessments.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery test–revised. Circle Pines: American Guidance Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R., McGrew, K., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson tests of achievement (3rd ed.). Itasca: Riverside.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The research reported here was supported by the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education, through a subaward to Florida State University from Grant R305F100005 to the Educational Testing Service as part of the Reading for Understanding Initiative. The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views of the Institute, the U.S. Department of Education, the Educational Testing Service, or Florida State University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yaacov Petscher.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Barbara Foorman, Yi-Chieh Wu, Jamie M. Quinn, and Yaacov Petscher are at the Florida Center for Reading Research at Florida State University.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Foorman, B.R., Wu, YC., Quinn, J.M. et al. How do latent decoding and language predict latent reading comprehension: across two years in grades 5, 7, and 9?. Read Writ 33, 2281–2309 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10043-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10043-3

Keywords

Navigation