Skip to main content
Log in

Applying a multiple group causal indicator modeling framework to the reading comprehension skills of third, seventh, and tenth grade students

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study demonstrates the utility of applying a causal indicator modeling framework to investigate important predictors of reading comprehension in third, seventh, and tenth grade students. The results indicated that a 4-factor multiple indicator multiple indicator cause (MIMIC) model of reading comprehension provided adequate fit at each grade level. This model included latent predictor constructs of decoding, verbal reasoning, nonverbal reasoning, and working memory and accounted for a large portion of the reading comprehension variance (73–87 %) across grade levels. Verbal reasoning contributed the most unique variance to reading comprehension at all grade levels. In addition, we fit a multiple group 4-factor MIMIC model to investigate the relative stability (or variability) of the predictor contributions to reading comprehension across development (i.e., grade levels). The results revealed that the contributions of verbal reasoning, nonverbal reasoning, and working memory to reading comprehension were stable across the three grade levels. Decoding was the only predictor that could not be constrained to be equal across grade levels. The contribution of decoding skills to reading comprehension was higher in third grade and then remained relatively stable between seventh and tenth grade. These findings illustrate the feasibility of using MIMIC models to explain individual differences in reading comprehension across the development of reading skills.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Lee, J. (2010). Kindergarten predictors of second versus eighth grade reading comprehension impairments. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(4), 332–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Little, T. D. (2006). Should the simple view of reading include a fluency component? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 933–958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 20–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asbell, S., Donders, J., Van Tubbergen, M., & Warschausky, S. (2010). Predictors of reading comprehension in children with cerebral palsy and typically developing children. Child Neuropsychology, 16, 313–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–87). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I., & McKeown, M. (1991). Conditions of vocabulary acquisition. In R. Barr, M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 789–814). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbarum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bollen, K. A., & Bauldry, S. (2011). Three Cs in measurement models: Causal indicators, composite indicators, and covariates. Psychological Methods, 16(3), 265–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bollen, K. A., & Davis, W. F. (2009). Causal indicator models: Identification, estimation, and testing. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 16(3), 498–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brace, Harcourt. (1997). Stanford achievement test (9th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (1998). Comprehension skill and inference-making ability: Issues of causality. In C. Hulme & R. Joshi (Eds.), Reading and spelling: Development and disorders (pp. 329–342). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (Eds.). (2007). Children’s comprehension problems in oral and written language: A cognitive perspective. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 31–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Adlof, S. M. (2005). Developmental changes in reading and reading disabilities. In H. W. Catts & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), Connections between language and reading disabilities (pp. 2025–2040). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, R., & Vellutino, F. R. (1997). Prediction of reading ability: A cross-validation study of the simple view of reading. Journal of Literacy Research, 29(1), 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 233–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corporation, Psychological. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI) manual. San Antonio, TX: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, L. E., & Scarborough, H. S. (2006). Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(3), 277–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(4), 422–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2002). Effects of phonological abilities and linguistic comprehension on the development of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6(1), 51–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diakidoy, I. N., Stylianou, P., Karefillidou, C., & Papageorgiou, P. (2005). The relationship between listening and reading comprehension of different types of text at increasing grade levels. Reading Psychology, 26, 55–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyer, L. G., & Katz, L. (1992). An examination of “the simple view of reading”. National Reading Conference Yearbook, 41, 169–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Florida Department of Education. (2006). Florida Comprehensive Assessment Test: Technical Report 2006. Tallahassee, FL. Retrieved March 15, 2012, from http://fcat.fldoe.org/pdf/fc06tech.pdf

  • Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, L. S., Bryant, V. J., Hamlett, C. L., & Lambert, W. (2012). First-grade cognitive abilities as long-term predictors of reading comprehension and disability status. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(3), 217–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaulin, C. A., & Campbell, T. F. (1994). Procedure for assessing verbal working memory in normal school-age children: Some preliminary data. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79(1), 55–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goff, D., Pratt, C., & Ong, B. (2005). The relations between children’s reading comprehension, working memory, language skills and components of reading decoding in a normal sample. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. RASE: Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3(4), 424–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1980). Bias in mental testing. New York, NY: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kershaw, S., & Schatschneider, C. (2012). A latent variable approach to the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25(2), 433–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Petscher, Y., Schatschneider, C., & Foorman, B. (2010). Does growth rate in oral reading fluency matter in predicting reading achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 652–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J., & Kane, M. J. (2012). Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(2), 302–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meade, A. W., & Lautenschlager, G. J. (2004). A comparison of item response theory and confirmatory factor analytic tests of measurement invariance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 568–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molloy, P. J. (1997). The role of individual differences in working memory in reading and listening comprehension in intermediate grade students. Doctoral Dissertation. Available from Dissertation Abstracts International, 2072A. (UMI No. AAMNN19626).

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2010). Mplus user’s guide. (6th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

  • Nagy, W. E., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second-grade readers and at-risk fourth-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Adams, J. W., Bowyer-Crane, C. A., & Snowling, M. J. (1999). Working memory deficits in poor comprehenders reflect underlying language impairments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73(2), 139–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(2), 91–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of single-word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 443–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SAS Institute Inc. (2012). Base SAS 9.2 utilities: Reference. Cary, NC: Author.

  • Schatschneider, C., Harrell, E., & Buck, J. (2007). An individual differences approach to the study of reading comprehension. In R. K. Wagner, A. E. Muse, & K. R. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 249–275). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seigneuric, A., & Ehrlich, M. F. (2005). Contribution of working memory capacity to children’s reading comprehension: A longitudinal investigation. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18(7–9), 617–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seigneuric, A., Ehrlich, M. F., Oakhill, J., & Yuill, N. (2000). Working memory resources and children’s reading comprehension. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 617–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., & Powell, J. S. (1983). Comprehending verbal comprehension. American Psychologist, 38, 878–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sticht, T. G., & James, J. H. (1984). Listening and reading. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 255–292). New York, NY: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Berninger, V. (1995). The role of working memory in skilled and less skilled readers’ comprehension. Intelligence, 21(1), 83–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Howell, M. (2001). Working memory, short-term memory, and speech rate as predictors of children’s reading performance at different ages. Journal or Educational Psychology, 93, 720–734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & O’Connor, R. (2009). The role of working memory and fluency practice on the reading comprehension of students who are dysfluent readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(6), 548–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tighe, E. L. (2012). An investigation of the dimensionality of morphological and vocabulary knowledge in Adult Basic Education students. Unpublished masters thesis. Florida State University.

  • Tighe, E. L., & Schatschneider, C. (2014). A dominance analysis approach to determining predictor importance in third, seventh, and tenth grade reading comprehension skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27(1), 101–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tilstra, J., McMaster, K., Van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., & Rapp, D. (2009). Simple but complex: Components of the simple view of reading across grade levels. Journal of Research in Reading, 32(4), 383–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tiu, R. D, Jr, Thompson, L. A., & Lewis, B. A. (2003). The role of IQ in a component model of reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(5), 424–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., Nettles, S., Howard, P., & Winterbottom, R. (2005). Brief report of a study to investigate the relationship between several brief measure of reading fluency and performance on the Florida Comprehensive Assessment Test-Reading in 4th, 6th, 8th, and 10th grades (Technical Report #6). Tallahassee, FL: Florida Center for Reading Research. Retrieved from: http://www.fcrr.org/TechnicalReports/Progress_monitoring_report.pdf

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiblmaier, H., Bentler, P. M., & Mair, P. (2011). Formative constructs implemented via common factors. Structural Equation Modeling, 18, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Tunmer, W. E., Jaccard, J. J., & Chen, R. (2007). Components of reading ability: Multivariate evidence for a convergent skills model of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(1), 3–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L., & van Leeuwe, J. (2008). Prediction of the development of reading comprehension: A longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 407–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K. (2013, January). A causal indicator model of reading comprehension. Paper presented at the Frontier Lecture Series at Texas A&M University, College Station, TX.

Download references

Acknowledgments

Support for this manuscript was provided by Grant Number P50 HD052120 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development awarded to the second and third authors. Support was also provided by Predoctoral Interdisciplinary Research Training Fellowships R305B04074 and R305B090021 from the Institute of Education Sciences awarded to the first author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elizabeth L. Tighe.

Appendix: Establishing measurement invariance in multiple group causal indicator models

Appendix: Establishing measurement invariance in multiple group causal indicator models

Measurement invariance (MI) refers to scores on a latent construct having the same operational definition across different groups, time points, or methods of measurement administration (Meade & Lautenschlager, 2004). To establish MI across the structural components of our three independent groups (third, seventh, and tenth grade), we followed a six-step process and inspected and compared model fit indices at each step (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002; Kline, 2011). It is important to note that the steps proceed in hierarchical fashion such that subsequent levels impose additional equivalence constraints and therefore, indicate stronger MI.

Step one is to ensure that the same general model fits reasonably well at all grade levels. As demonstrated in our results section, a 4-factor MIMIC model provided adequate fit at each grade level. Inspecting the model fit indices and factor loadings of the observed measures onto the latent constructs also showed similar loadings across the three grades. Thus, we can explore building a multiple group MIMIC model.

Step two is to obtain a configural or baseline model (labeled M0 in Table 6) by fitting the 4-factor MIMIC model across the three grades without any invariances (or cross-group equality constraints). By default for a multiple group model, Mplus imposes cross-group equality constraints on factor loadings and intercepts. We are unable to relax the cross-group equality constraints on intercepts because this would lead to an unidentified model. However, we were able to run a baseline model that freed up the default cross-group constraints on factor loadings. This model is referred to as a baseline model (M0) because this model was compared with subsequent models in our later steps of testing for MI. Comparing Chi square values from the three models (one per grade) and M0 demonstrated that the baseline model is merely the sum of Chi square values and degrees of freedom from each of the individual models.

Table 6 Testing for multiple group measurement invariance of the structural components

Step three is to test for construct-level metric invariance or equal factor loadings. To achieve this, we constrained all unstandardized factor loadings to be equal across the three groups (Table 6). A Chi square difference test between this model (M1) and the baseline model (M0) revealed a non-significant difference [M1–M0 χ2(12) = 15.39, p = .221]. Inspection of the model fit indices revealed an increase in RMSEA and TLI values in the constrained M1 model, indicating better model fit. Therefore, we established invariance of factor loadings, which indicates that the latent constructs (decoding, verbal reasoning, nonverbal reasoning, and working memory) are equivalent across grades.

Step four is to test for the equivalence of construct variances and covariances. To do this, we imposed constraints on factor loadings and factor variances and covariances (Table 6). A Chi square difference test between this model (M2) and the Step three model (M1) revealed no significant differences [M2–M1 χ2(8) = 1.37, p = .994]. The model fit indices show increased model fit for the RMSEA, TLI, and CFI values. Thus, cross-group equivalence of factor variances and covariances is supported.

Step five is to test for invariance of the correlations among our latent constructs (decoding, verbal reasoning, nonverbal reasoning, and working memory). To do this, cross-group equality constraints were imposed on factor loadings, factor covariances, and correlations (Table 6). A Chi square difference test between this model (M3) and the model in Step four (M2) revealed no significant differences [M3–M2 χ2(12) = 18.05, p = .114]. Inspection of the model fit indices revealed increased model fit for the RMSEA and TLI values. Thus, cross-group equivalence of predictor correlations is supported.

Step six is to test for invariance of the residual variances. To do this, cross-group equality constraints were enforced on factor loadings, factor variances and covariances, construct correlations, and residual variances (Table 6). A Chi square difference test between this model (M4) and the model in Step five (M3) revealed a significant difference [M4–M3(22) = 48.76, p < .001]. Inspection of the model fit indices demonstrated decreased model fit for the RMSEA, CFI, and TLI values. Although model fit was still in an overall acceptable range (Hu & Bentler, 1998), we could not declare cross-group equivalence of residual variances. Thus, we utilized model M3 as our baseline model to test for the stability of the estimates of the four causal indicator pathways.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tighe, E.L., Wagner, R.K. & Schatschneider, C. Applying a multiple group causal indicator modeling framework to the reading comprehension skills of third, seventh, and tenth grade students. Read Writ 28, 439–466 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9532-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9532-1

Keywords

Navigation