Skip to main content
Log in

Lexical properties influencing visual word recognition in Hangul

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined how lexical properties, such as word frequency, word length, and morphological features, affect the word recognition of Korean Hangul among adult readers. Ninety-four native Korean students performed a lexical decision task on disyllabic and trisyllabic words and nonwords. Results of cross-classified and hierarchical linear modeling showed a significant frequency effect but null effects of word length and their interactions on word recognition. The null syllable effect might be contributable to the block structure of syllables, which does not require syllabic decomposition in recognition. Given that Chinese-derived Sino-Korean words account for about 70% of the Korean lexicon, morphological features embedded in the word were examined in order to further understand the morphological undercurrent of multisyllabic word recognition. Word type (bound-morphemic words vs. compound words), word origin (native or semi-native words vs. Chinese-derived words), and morphemic transparency (opaque morpheme vs. transparent morpheme) were significant predictors of the speed of visual word recognition. An analysis of frequency split showed that compound words and morpheme transparency facilitated word recognition for high frequency words, while the number of morphemes within the word, Korean native and semi-native words, and morphemic transparency facilitated word recognition for low frequency words. These results suggest that high and low frequency words function differently according to morphological information available within the multisyllabic word in visual word recognition in Hangul.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Although this study examined lexical decision data, a small number of studies reporting naming data was reviewed in this article only when naming data were presented in comparison to lexical decision data in the studies reviewed. In addition, the literature review is based on adult data because children’s reading is still in the (transient) developmental stage, as Beyersmann, Castles, and Coltheart (2012) indicate that reading automization does not appear until a relatively late stage in reading development.

  2. In this article, curly brackets {} are used for orthographic representations and angle brackets < > for semantic explanations.

  3. The word frequency measure, based on a corpus of three million words that were published in print materials since 1990, includes frequencies of segments (i.e., consonants and vowels), syllables, words, particles, suffixes, and phrases.

  4. The determination of the morphological properties of the stimuli was complicated to some degree. Some Korean words have a mixture of a native Korean word and a Sino-Korean word; for example, The word {강물} <river (water)> has one Chinese-origin syllable {강} <river> and another native one-syllable word {물} <water> in the first and the second position, respectively. We classified the mixture of a Korean morpheme and a Sino-Korean morpheme as a semi-Korean word because the focus of the classification was whether the word was of Chinese-origin or not in order to tease apart the role of morphological information.

  5. To be consistent with the coding scheme for the determination of the word origin, the mixture of an opaque and a transparent morpheme was considered opaque because the purpose of the study was to examine the role of clear morphological information.

References

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bae, S., Park, K., Lee, H.-W., & Yi, K. (2016). The mono-syllabic word inferiority effect within korean word recognition. The Journal of Linguistic Science, 77, 109–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., Spieler, D. H., & Yap, M. J. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 283–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barton, J. J. S., Hanif, H. M., Björnström, L. E., & Hills, C. (2014). The word-length effect in reading: A review. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 31, 378–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beyersmann, E., Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2012). Morphological processing during visual word recognition in developing readers: Evidence from masked priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1306–1326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M., Buchmeier, M., Conrad, M., Jacobs, A. M., Bölte, J., & Böhl, A. (2011). The word frequency effect: A review of recent developments and implications for the choice of frequency estimates in German. Experimental Psychology, 58, 412–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E. (2016). The impact of word prevalence on lexical decision times: Evidence from the Dutch lexicon project 2. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 441–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carreiras, M., Álvarez, C. J., & de Vega, M. (1993). Syllable frequency and visual word recognition in Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 766–780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2004). Replicating syllable-frequency effects in Spanish in German: One more challenge to computational models of visual word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 369–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diependaele, K., Duñabeitia, J. A., Morris, J., & Keuleers, E. (2011). Fast morphological effects in first and second language word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 344–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrand, L., & New, B. (2003). Syllabic length effects in visual word recognition and naming. Acta Psychologica, 113, 167–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foster, K. I., & Foster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 35, 116–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, H. S. (2005). The frequency of modern Korean use. Seoul: National Institute of the Korean Language.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K. H. (1993). A discourse on the Korean lexicon. Seoul: Jibmoondang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, E.-B. (1980). kwuk-e sacen ehwi uy lyupyel kwusenghwa lo pon hanca-e uy cwung-yoto wa kyoyuk mwuncey. The Society for Korean Language & Literary Research, 8, 136–141. (in Korean).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, C., Lee, Y., & Kim, T. H. (2016). The exploration of the effects of word frequency and word length on Korean word recognition. Journal of the Korea Academia-Industrial Cooperation Society, 17(1), 54–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, W. S., & Foster, K. I. (2004). Serial mechanisms in lexical access: The rank hypothesis. Psychological Review, 111, 721–756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • New, B., Ferrand, L., Pallier, C., & Brysbaert, M. (2006). Reexamining the word length effect in visual word recognition: New evidence from the English Lexicon Project. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 45–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oh, J. H., Choi, M. G., Yi, H. W., & Lee, C. H. (2007). The effects of morphemes on Korean word recognition: Revealed on the frequency and length effects. Third International Conference on Natural Computation, 5, 531–535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pae, H. K., Yi, K., & Bae, S. (2020). More than an alphabet: Linguistic features of Korean and their influences on word recognition of Hangul. Literacy and Written Languages, 22, 223–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, K. (1993). Mental code involved in Hangul word recognition. Korean Journal of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, 5, 40–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (1998). Effects of syllable frequency and syllable neighborhood frequency in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 134–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., & Congdon, R. (2010). HLM 7 hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling. Chicago: Scientific Software International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuel, A. G., van Santen, J. P., & Johnston, J. C. (1982). Length effects in word perception: We is better than I but worse than you or them. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 91–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schilling, H. H., Rayner, K., & Chumbley, J. I. (1998). Comparing naming, lexical decision, and eye fixation times: Word frequency effects and individual differences. Memory and Cognition, 26, 1270–1281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, G. B., & Kang, H. (1994). The flexible use of phonological information in word recognition in Korean. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 319–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spinelli, D., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Mancini, M., Martelli, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2005). Length effect in word naming in reading: Role of reading experience and reading deficit in Italian readers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 217–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, I., & Taylor, M. M. (2014). Writing and literacy in Chinese, Korean, and Japanese (revised ed.). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yan, T., & Tourangeau, R. (2008). Fast times and easy questions: The effects of age, experience and question complexity on web survey response times. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 51–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yap, M. J., & Balota, D. A. (2009). Visual word recognition of multisyllabic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 502–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yi, K. (2003). The effects of word types on word recognition in Korean. Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, 15(4), 479–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yi, K., Koo, M.-M., Nam, K., Park, K., Park, T., Bae, S., et al. (2017). The Korean lexicon project: A lexical decision study on 30,930 Korean words and nonwords. The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, 29(4), 395–410. (in Korean).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yi, K., & Pak, H. (1997). The restoration of deep syllables and the role of syllables in Korean speech perception. The Korean Journal of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, 9, 73–94. (in Korean).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Republic of Korean’s Ministry of Education and the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2016S1A5A2A03927124).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Hye K. Pae or Kwangoh Yi.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Stimuli Used

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pae, H.K., Bae, S. & Yi, K. Lexical properties influencing visual word recognition in Hangul. Read Writ 33, 2391–2412 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10042-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10042-4

Keywords

Navigation