Skip to main content
Log in

Morphosyntax in poor comprehenders

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Children described as poor comprehenders (PCs) have reading comprehension difficulties in spite of adequate word reading abilities. PCs are known to display weakness with semantics and higher-level aspects of oral language, but less is known about their grammatical skills, especially with regard to morphosyntax. The purpose of this study was to examine morphosyntax in fourth grade PCs and typically developing readers (TDs), using three experimental tasks involving finiteness marking. Participants also completed standardized, norm-referenced assessments of phonological memory, vocabulary, and broader language skills. PCs displayed weakness relative to TDs on all three morphosyntax tasks and on every other assessment of oral language except phonological memory, as indexed by nonword repetition. These findings help to clarify the linguistic profile of PCs, suggesting that their language weaknesses include grammatical weaknesses that cannot be fully explained by semantic factors. Because finiteness markers are usually mastered prior to formal schooling in typical development, we call for future studies to examine whether assessments of morphosyntax could be used for the early identification of children at risk for future reading comprehension difficulty.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Not all irregular past tense verbs are overtly marked for finiteness. Some irregular past tense verbs (e.g., hit, cut) are phonologically and orthographically identical to their present tense forms.

  2. Although teachers used progress-monitoring scores to distribute consent forms, the scores were not provided to the examiners for analysis in this study.

  3. Because this study examined knowledge of standard American English morphosyntax, it was not appropriate to include speakers of dialects that follow different grammatical rules, such as AAE. Therefore, audio recordings of the Recalling Sentences and Formulated Sentences subtests of the CELF-4 were checked by a trained research assistant for the presence of features of AAE. No PCs displayed these features, but one member of the TD group did and was subsequently excluded from further analyses.

  4. We note that under a different analysis, “Where do a boy like to play” could be considered an error of omission because the bare-stem form of DO is phonologically identical to the third-person singular form. DO also differs from BE because it is inserted to mark tense and agreement for the formation of questions, rather than being generated in the matrix clause and moved. The following example demonstrates this difference.

    figure a

References

  • Adlof, S. M., & Catts, H. W. (2007). Classification of children with poor reading comprehension. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Boston, MA.

  • Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Lee, J. (2010). Kindergarten predictors of second vs. eighth grade reading comprehension impairments. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 332–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Little, T. D. (2006). Should the simple view of reading include a fluency component? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 933–958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adlof, S. M., & Perfetti, C. A. (2013). Individual differences in word learning and reading ability. In A. Stone, E. Silliman, B. Ehren, & G. Wallach (Eds.), Handbook of language and literacy development and disorders (2nd ed., pp. 246–264). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atchley, R. A., Rice, M. L., Betz, S. K., Kwasney, K. M., Sereno, J. M., & Jongman, A. (2006). A comparison of semantic and syntactic event related potentials generated by children and adults. Brain and Language, 99, 236–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, G., Slonims, V., Siminoff, E., & Dworzynski, K. (2011). Impairment in non-word repetition: A marker for language impairment or reading impairment? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 53, 711–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bedore, L., & Leonard, L. (1998). Specific language impairment and grammatical morphology: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1185–1192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Betz, S. K. (2005). Language based event-related potentials in children with and without specific language impairment. Doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas. ISBN: 0542487764.

  • Botting, N., Simkin, Z., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Associated reading skills in children with a history of specific language impairment. Reading and Writing, 19, 77–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (1997). Test of nonverbal intelligence-3. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K. (2003). Text comprehension and its relation to coherence and cohesion in children’s fictional narratives. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 335–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (1996). The nature of the relationship between comprehension skill and the ability to tell a story. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 187–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2006). Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension difficulties. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 683–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2011). Matthew effects in young readers: Reading comprehension and reading experience aid vocabulary development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 431–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2000). Phonological skills and comprehension failure: A test of the phonological processing deficit hypothesis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13, 31–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Towse, A. S. (2008). To get hold of the wrong end of the stick: Reasons for poor idiom understanding in children with reading comprehension difficulties. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 1538–1549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2006). Language deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 278–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2005). Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1378–1396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, R. S., & Vellutino, F. R. (1997). Prediction of reading ability: A cross-validation study of the simple view of reading. Journal of Literacy Research, 29(1), 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cragg, L., & Nation, K. (2006). Exploring written narrative in children with poor reading comprehension. Educational Psychology, 26, 55–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test (4th ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elwer, A., Keenan, J. M., Olson, R. K., Byrne, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2013). Longitudinal stability and predictors of poor oral comprehenders and poor decoders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 497–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flax, J. F., Realpe-Bonilla, T., Hirsch, L. S., Brzustowitz, L. M., Bartlett, C. W., & Tallal, P. (2003). Specific language impairment in families: Evidence for co-occurrence with reading impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 530–543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education (RASE), 7, 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L., Snowling, M., & Clarke, P. (2013). Accessing, integrating, and inhibiting word meaning in poor comprehenders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 177–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2, 127–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, B. G. (2008). Direct RT research software (v2008). New York, NY: Empirisoft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landi, N. (2010). An examination of the relationship between reading comprehension, higher-level, and lower-level reading sub-skills in adults. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 701–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landi, N., & Perfetti, C. A. (2007). An electrophysiological investigation of semantic and phonological processing in skilled and less-skilled comprehenders. Brain and Language, 102, 30–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malone, L. M., Cabili, C., Henderson, J., Esposito, A.M., Coolahan, K., Henke, J., … & Boller, K. (2010). Compendium of student, teacher, and classroom measures used in NCEE evaluations of educational interventions. Technical details, measure profiles, and glossary (appendices AG) (NCEE 2010-4013) (Vol. II). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, US Department of Education.

  • Marchman, V. A., Wulfeck, B., & Ellis Weismer, S. (1999). Morphological productivity in children with normal language and SLI: A study of the English past tense. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 206–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, C. M., & Nation, K. (2003). Individual differences in semantic and structural errors in children’s memory for sentences. Educational and Child Psychology, 20, 7–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, C. A., Leonard, L. B., & Finneran, D. (2008). Grammaticality judgements in adolescents with and without language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 346–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moe, A. J., Hopkins, C. J., & Rush, R. T. (1982). The vocabulary of first-grade children. Springfield, IL: Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Adams, J. W., Bowyer-Crane, C. A., & Snowling, M. J. (1999). Working memory deficits in poor comprehenders reflect underlying language impairments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 139–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Clarke, P., Marshall, C. M., & Durand, M. (2004). Hidden language impairments in children: Parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language impairments? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 199–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Cocksey, J., Taylor, J. S. H., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). A longitudinal investigation of early reading and language skills in children with poor reading comprehension. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 1031–1039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (1997). Assessing reading difficulties: The validity and utility of current measures of reading skill. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 359–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1998). Semantic processing and the development of word-recognition skills: Evidence from children with reading comprehension difficulties. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 85–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1999). Developmental differences in sensitivity to semantic relations among good and poor comprehenders: Evidence from semantic priming. Cognition, 70, B1–B13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2000). Factors influencing syntactic awareness skills in normal readers and poor comprehenders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 229–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Snowling, M. J., & Clarke, P. (2005). Production of the English past tense by children with language comprehension impairments. Journal of Child Language, 32, 117–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J., Yuill, N., & Parkin, A. J. (1986). On the nature of the difference between skilled and less-skilled comprehenders. Journal of Research in Reading, 9, 80–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor-Williams, K., & Fey, M. E. (2007). Recast density and acquisition of novel irregular past tense verbs. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1029–1047.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redmond, S. M., & Rice, M. L. (2001). Detection of irregular verb violations by children with and without SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 655–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., Hoffman, L., & Wexler, K. (2009). Judgments of omitted BE and DO in questions as extended finiteness clinical markers of SLI to fifteen years: A study of growth and asymptote. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 1417–1433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., Tomblin, J. B., Hoffman, L., Richman, W. A., & Marquis, J. (2004). Grammatical tense deficits in children with SLI and nonspecific language impairment: Relationships with nonverbal IQ over time. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 816–834.

  • Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (2001). Rice–Wexler test of early grammatical impairment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of Extended Optional Infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Hershberger, S. (1998). Tense over time: The longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1412–1431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Redmond, S. M. (1999). Grammaticality judgments of an extended optional infinitive grammar: Evidence from English speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 943–961.

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2006). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stothard, S. E., & Hulme, C. (1992). Reading comprehension difficulties in children: The role of language comprehension and working memory skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 4, 245–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stothard, S. E., & Hulme, C. (1995). A comparison of phonological skills in children with reading comprehension difficulties and children with decoding difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 399–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, J. B., Records, N., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tong, X., Deacon, S. H., & Cain, K. (2014). Morphological and syntactic awareness in poor comprehenders: Another piece of the puzzle. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 22–33.

  • Tong, X., Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., Cain, K., & Parrila, R. (2011). Morphological awareness: A key to understanding poor reading comprehension in English. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(3), 523–534.

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. T., Cassidy, J., & Samuels, S. J. (2001). Group reading assessment and diagnostic evaluation. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wulfeck, B., Bates, E., Krupa-Kwiatkowski, M., & Saltzman, D. (2004). Grammaticality sensitivity in children with early focal brain injury and children with specific language impairment. Brain and Language, 88, 215–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuill, N., & Joscelyne, T. (1988). Effects of organizational cues and strategies on good and poor comprehenders’ story understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 152–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuill, N., & Oakhill, J. (1991). Children’s problems in text comprehension: An experimental investigation. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeno, S. M., Ivens, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The educator’s word frequency guide. Brewster, NY: TASA.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported, in part, by funds from the National Institutes of Health (DC009133), the International Reading Association, and the American Speech-Language-Hearing Foundation. The contents of this article are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the funding agencies. This study was conducted as part of the first author’s doctoral studies at the University of Kansas, under the direction of the second author and committee members Diane Loeb, Mabel Rice, Sarah Rosen, and Holly Storkel. The authors would like to thank the students as well as parents and teachers from USD 497 and 501 for their participation in this study. Thanks to April Best, Stacy Cates, and Jennifer Shipp for assistance with standardized testing, to Jill Hoover for assistance with transcription reliability, David Hahn for assistance with picture stimulus preparation, and Steven Luke for consultation on statistical analyses.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Suzanne M. Adlof.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Adlof, S.M., Catts, H.W. Morphosyntax in poor comprehenders. Read Writ 28, 1051–1070 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9562-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9562-3

Keywords

Navigation