Skip to main content
Log in

Parent–child conversations about letters and pictures

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Learning about letters, and how they differ from pictures, is one important aspect of a young child’s print awareness. To test the hypothesis that parent speech provides children with information about these differences, we studied parent–child conversations in CHILDES (MacWhinney, 2000). We found that parents talk to their young children about letters, differentiating them from pictures, by 1–2 years of age and that some of these conversational patterns change across the preschool years in ways that emphasize important features of letters, such as their shape. We also found that children talk about letters and pictures in distinct ways, suggesting an implicit understanding of some of the differences between letters and pictures at an early age. Some differences in parent–child conversations about letters were found as a function of socioeconomic status: Lower SES families appeared to focus more on alphabetic order than higher SES families. The general letter knowledge expressed in these conversations suggests that everyday interactions are an important component of the home literacy environment and that they differ, in some respects, as a function of child age and family background.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Corpora that were included were: Bates, Bernstein, Bliss, Bloom70, Bloom73, Bohannon, Brown, Clark, Cornell, Demetras1, Demetras2, Feldman, Gleason, Haggerty, Hall, Higginson, HSLLD, Kuczaj, MacWhinney, Morisset, Nelson, New England, Post, Providence, Sachs, Snow, Suppes, Tardiff, Valian, VanHouten, VanKleeck, and Warren. The corpora are available for download on the CHILDES website at http://childes.psy.cmu.edu/ (MacWhinney, 2000).

  2. Because there were so few instances of utterances like write a picture, there were no inappropriate utterances for picture in some age ranges. This resulted in near-perfect coefficients and a large standard error for the effect of referent. This is a familiar problem for models that use the Wald statistic, as multilevel models do (Menard, 2002).

References

  • Aram, D., & Biron, S. (2004). Joint storybook reading and joint writing interventions among low SES preschoolers: Differential contributions to early literacy. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 588–610. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.10.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D., & Levin, I. (2004). Mother–child joint writing and storybook reading: Relations with literacy among low SES kindergartners. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 48, 202–224. doi:10.1353/mpq.2002.0005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A. (2000). Do preschool orthographic skills contribute to prevention of reading? In N. Badian (Ed.), Prediction and prevention of reading failure. Timonium, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. (1995). Children talk about the mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2011). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-39. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.

  • Clay, M. (2000). Concepts of print: What have children learned about the way we print language. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeBaryshe, B. D. (1995). Maternal belief systems: Linchpin in the home reading process. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, 1–20. doi:10.1016/0193-3973(95)90013-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. O. (Eds.). (2001). Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L., Nunes, R. S., Willows, D., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, M. A., Bell, M., Shaw, D., Moretti, S., & Page, J. (2006). Letter names, letter sounds and phonological awareness: An examination of kindergarten children and of letters across children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 959–989. doi:10.1007/s11145-006-9026-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, M. A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 65–75. doi:10.1037/h0087330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgerald, J., Spiegel, D. L., & Cunningham, J. W. (1991). The relationship between parent literacy level and perceptions of emergent literacy. Journal of Literacy Research, 23, 191–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallerani, C. M., Saylor, M. M., & Adwar, S. (2009). Mother–infant conversation about absent things. Language Learning and Development, 5, 282–293. doi:10.1080/15475440902897604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hood, M., Conlon, E., & Andrews, G. (2008). Preschool home literacy practices and children’s literacy development: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 252–271. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., Snowling, M., Caravolas, M., & Carroll, J. (2005). Phonological skills are (probably) one cause of success in learning to read: A comment on Castles and Coltheart. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 351–365. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0904_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434–446. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavine, L. O. (1977). Differentiation of letter like forms in prereading children. Developmental Psychology, 13, 89–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., & Bus, A. G. (2003). How is emergent writing based on drawing? Analyses of children’s products and their sorting by children and mothers. Developmental Psychology, 39, 891–905.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B. A., Gong, Z., Hessels, S., Evans, M. A., & Jared, D. (2006). Understanding print: Early development and the contributions of home literacy experiences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 63–93. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Lin, D., Liu, P. D., Aram, D., Levin, I., Cho, J. et al. (2011). The ABC’s of Chinese: Maternal mediation of Pinyin for Chinese children’s early literacy skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, published online: doi:10.1007/s11145-010-9270-y.

  • Menard, S. (2002). Applied logistic regression analysis (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Early Literacy Panel. (2004). Report on a synthesis of early predictors of reading. Lexington, KY: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, M. M., Hood, M., & Neumann, D. L. (2008). The scaffolding of emergent literacy skills in the home environment: A case study. Early Childhood Education Journal, 36, 313–319. doi:10.1007/s10643-008-0291-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, B. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2009). Variations in the home literacy environment of preschool children: A cluster analytic approach. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 146–174. doi:10.1080/10888430902769533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puranik, C. S., Lonigan, C. J., & Kim, Y. S. (2011). Contributions of emergent literacy skills to name writing, letter writing, and spelling in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.03.002.

  • Raikes, H., Pan, B., Luze, G., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Brooks-Gunn, J., Constantine, J., et al. (2006). Mother–child book reading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development, 77, 924–953. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00911.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robins, S., & Treiman, R. (2009). Talking about writing: What we can learn from conversations between parents and their young children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 463–484. doi:10.1017/S0142716409090237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 445–460. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. (2000). Progress in understanding reading. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934–947. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swingley, D., & Fernald, A. (2002). Recognition of words referring to present and absent objects by 24-month-olds. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 39–56. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Cohen, J., Mulqueeny, K., Kessler, B., & Schechtman, S. (2007). Young children’s knowledge about printed names. Child Development, 78, 1458–1471. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01077.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsch, J. G., Sullivan, A., & Justice, L. M. (2003). That’s my letter!: What preschoolers’ name writing tells us about emergent literacy knowledge. Journal of Literacy Research, 35, 757–776. doi:10.1207/s15548430jlr3502_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848–872. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.00848.x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Worden, P. E., & Boettcher, W. (1990). Young children’s acquisition of alphabet knowledge. Journal of Reading Behavior, 22, 277–295. doi:10.1080/10862969009547711.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zucker, T. A., Justice, L. M., & Piasta, S. B. (2009). Prekindergarten teachers’ verbal references to print during classroom-based, large-group shared reading. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 40, 376–392. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2009/08-0059).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by NICHD Grant HD051610. We thank Michelle Lindblom, who performed the reliability coding; Brett Kessler, who performed the searches of CHILDES; Lindsey Clasen, who helped with the initial coding; and the members of the Reading and Language Lab, who provided feedback on a draft of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sarah Robins.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Robins, S., Treiman, R., Rosales, N. et al. Parent–child conversations about letters and pictures. Read Writ 25, 2039–2059 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9344-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9344-5

Keywords

Navigation