Abstract
Orienting attention by social gaze cues shares some characteristics with orienting attention by non-social arrow cues, but it is unclear whether they rely on similar neural mechanisms. The present ALE-meta-analysis assessed the pattern of brain activation reported in 40 single experiments (18 with arrows, 22 with gaze), with a total number of 806 participants. Our findings show that the network for orienting attention by social gaze and by non-social arrow cues is in part functionally segregated. Orienting by both types of cues relies on the activity of brain regions involved in endogenous attention (the superior frontal gyrus). Importantly, only orienting by gaze cues was also associated with the activity of brain regions involved in exogenous attention (medial frontal gyrus), processing gaze, and mental state attribution (superior temporal sulcus, temporoparietal junction).
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
The ability to shift attention based on the direction of eye gaze of another person is at the core of social attention (e.g., Baron-Cohen, 1995; Frith & Frith, 2001) and it has been extensively investigated using a variant of the standard attentional cueing paradigm (Posner, 1980). In this variant, the central symbolic cue is replaced by a face gazing left or right, and participants respond as quickly as possible to a peripheral target presented shortly after the gaze cue (Friesen & Kingstone, 1998). When gaze direction is not informative of where the target appears (i.e., 50% cue validity), a typical pattern of faster responses to targets presented at the spatial location looked at by the face (i.e., valid, or congruent cue) compared to targets presented at the opposite spatial location (i.e., invalid or incongruent cue) is observed (see McKay et al., 2021, for a recent review of gaze cueing effects). Because this effect is fast, as it occurs within 200 ms from the cues, and because it is observed with non-informative and even counter-informative central cues, it has been taken to indicate that it relies on exogenous mechanisms for orienting attention. This is because to elicit endogenous shifts of attention with purely symbolic cues, such as when a central stimulus characteristic is arbitrarily associated with a spatial location (e.g., a yellow circle indicates left and a blue circle indicates right), the cue needs to be predictive of target location and the SOA needs to be longer (> 300 ms) to engender cueing (e.g., Funes et al., 2007; Dodd & Wilson, 2009). Considering the social and biological relevance of faces, it had been originally proposed that orienting to eye gaze represents a unique attentional process that is qualitatively distinct from orienting based on other symbolic, central cues (e.g., Langton & Bruce, 1999; Driver et al., 1999; Friesen & Kingstone, 1998). However, this proposal is challenged by evidence of similar cueing effects observed with central, non-social cues such as arrow-cues (Hommel et al., 2001; Ristic et al., 2002; Tipples, 2002). Therefore, that gaze and arrow central cues shift attention even when they are not predictive of target location, with short SOAs suggesting that these cues may involve shifting attention that shares some characteristics of reflexive orienting typically observed with peripheral cues. Importantly, arrow and gaze also produce cueing effects that are greater than the summed effects of reflexive and volitional orienting. Accordingly, these effects have been attributed to gaze and arrows being perceptually asymmetrical signals or being overlearned directional signals, not requiring interpretation, which would challenge a dichotomous distinction in exogenous and endogenous attention. Indeed, in a recent review, Dalmaso et al. (2020) highlights that gaze and arrow cues may be associated with different forms of automatic orienting of attention.
Although the mechanisms underlying gaze and arrow cueing effects on attention have been extensively investigated (see Chica et al., 2014 for a review), it is still debated to what extent cueing effects elicited by central gaze and arrow cues rely on similar neural underpinnings. On one hand, it has been proposed that orienting attention based on gaze and arrow cues is qualitatively similar as both rely upon the dorsal and ventral frontoparietal networks, and the only difference is quantitative as orienting by gaze cues involves the lateral occipital regions (e.g., Tipper et al., 2008). Indeed, frontoparietal brain networks play an important role in spatial attentional orienting, and the debate is on whether there is a single attentional system supporting both endogenous and exogenous attention, or two anatomically and functionally different attentional systems (for a review see Chica et al., 2014). For instance, Corbetta et al. (2008) put forward the hypothesis of a bilateral, dorsal frontoparietal network for orienting both endogenous and exogenous attention and of a right-lateralized, ventral frontoparietal counterpart for reorienting to task-relevant events. The dorsal attention system is associated with covert and overt shifts of spatial attention, eye movements, and hand-eye coordination (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002) and includes regions in the frontal eye fields, ventral premotor cortex, superior parietal lobule, intraparietal sulcus, and motion-sensitive middle temporal area (MT+). Recent findings from fMRI studies show positive correlations between the activity of these regions (Laufs et al., 2003; Fox et al., 2005, 2006; Vincent et al., 2006). On the other hand, a key difference between gaze and arrows is that only orienting attention based on observed gaze direction is taken to indicate that we spontaneously attribute a mental state to others as we understand that a person looks at what they are interested in (see Senju et al., 2004; Pecchinenda & Petrucci, 2021). Therefore, the neural mechanisms involved in orienting attention by gaze cues should rely also on brain areas involved in mental state attribution and Theory of Mind (e.g., Calder et al., 2002) – that is the human ability to ascribe mental states to others. In contrast, the neural mechanisms involved in orienting attention based on arrow cues should not. Indeed, a large network of brain regions has been associated with mental state attribution and social cognition, including the medial prefrontal cortex, posterior superior temporal sulcus, temporoparietal junction (Caruana et al., 2015; Redcay et al., 2012), intraparietal sulcus (Saito et al., 2010), occipital gyrus (Oberwelland et al., 2016), precuneus, insula, and amygdala (Caruana et al., 2015).
In sum, orienting by gaze and arrow cues shares some characteristics of endogenous (i.e., it occurs with central cues) and some of exogenous (i.e., short SOA, it occurs even with non-predictive cues) cueing, but the question of whether orienting by social and non-social cues differs regarding the neural mechanisms involved is still unresolved. However, this can be addressed by performing a coordinate-based Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) meta-analysis on the available fMRI studies as it provides a synthesis of previous results, and it allows resolving conflicting views while overcoming the limitations of single studies (e.g., small sample size, low power, and generalization). Importantly, as we are interested in higher order processes (i.e., orienting by gaze and arrow cues) and to maximise the number of studies that could be included in the ALE meta-analysis, we are not distinguishing between predictive and non-predictive cues. That is, for both gaze and arrow cues, we are considering converging evidence about the brain activation maps when gaze and arrow cues are predictive as well as when they are non-predictive of target location. This strategy is in line with recommendations by Muller et al. (2018). Albeit meta-analyses of the neural networks involved in social attention are available, to our knowledge a direct comparison of the neural substrates of orienting attention based on gaze and arrow cues is missing.
To this aim, we conducted an ALE-meta-analysis with the following main purposes: (1) to provide a synthesis of the main brain networks involved in gaze and arrow cueing; (2) to test the hypothesis that different neural networks underlie orienting attention based on gaze and arrow cues. Even if this latter issue has been investigated using behavioural and EEG measures (e.g., Ristic et al., 2002, Hietanen et al., 2008; Brignani et al., 2009), no coordinate-based meta-analysis has so far been performed.
Meta-Analysis
Selection Criteria
We conducted a systematic review of the literature according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses, (PRISMA, Page et al., 2021; see Appendix Table 5 for the PRISMA Checklist on how PRISMA guidelines were followed). We searched the literature using different databases (i.e., PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Science) for articles published up to December 2021 using the following search syntax: (a) “fMRI” AND “attention” AND “arrow” (PubMed: 48; Scopus: 1072, Web of Science: 46); (b) “fMRI” AND “attention” AND “gaze” (PubMed: 214; Scopus: 258; Web of Science: 218). The search was performed on whole articles published in English. In total, 1856 potential articles were found (PubMed: 262; Scopus: 1330; Web of Science: 264). After duplicates were removed (N = 257), 1599 articles were screened according to the following a priori criteria:
Inclusion criteria:
-
1.
Articles reporting whole-brain analyses performed using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI);
-
2.
Articles in which coordinates of activation foci were provided either in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) or in Talairach reference space;
-
3.
fMRI studies with a visuo-perceptual/motor control condition to exclude all activations not directly related to visual orienting;
-
4.
Studies with non-pathological groups;
-
5.
Studies involving attentional orienting by central cues;
-
6.
Group studies.
Exclusion criteria:
-
1.
Studies reporting only results from region of interest (ROI) analyses;
-
2.
Studies reporting only results of multivariate analyses (e.g., representational similarity or decoding analyses) or connectivity analyses (e.g., psychophysiological interaction analyses or dynamic causal modelling);
-
3.
Studies involving pharmacological manipulations, psychotherapeutic interventions, or other manipulations of the participants’ psychophysical conditions;
-
4.
Studies using emotional stimuli and auditory cues to orient attention;
-
5.
Reviews, book chapters, books, conference papers, and meta-analyses;
-
6.
Single case reports;
-
7.
Studies contrasting experimental conditions with a low-level baseline (e.g., rest condition);
-
8.
Studies in which arrow and gaze stimuli were used only for passive viewing;
-
9.
Studies using a judgement task on cue direction;
-
10.
Studies not reporting contrasts specific for each type of cue (i.e., arrow, gaze).
Accordingly, 248 articles were selected to be assessed for eligibility. After examining the whole content, 20 articles satisfied the adopted criteria and were included in the ALE meta-analysis (see Fig. 1). One investigator conducted the search literature, performed the removal of duplicates, and selected articles based on inclusion criteria. The other investigators double-checked the final selection.
The meta-analysis was performed on 40 single experiments, involving a total number of 806 participants, extracted from the 20 selected papers. All experiments included contrasting attentional orienting to a high-level control condition (e.g., word-related processing, visuo-perceptual processing), to assess that all activations were directly related to the central spatial cueing. There were 18 experiments with arrow cues from 11 papers, and 22 experiments with gaze cues from 11 papers (in two papers, both arrow and gaze cues were used).
Table 1 provides the complete list of contrasts included in the meta-analysis, along with details about (1) the paper from which they were extracted; (2) the ALE analysis in which they were included; (3) the type of cue used.
Importantly, although all included experiments used similar methodologies, they also differed in some aspects. More specifically, in some experiments, only one type of cue was used (e.g., arrow cues: Dombert et al., 2016; Mao et al., 2007; Natale et al., 2009; Noesselt et al., 2002; Peelen et al., 2004; Small et al., 2003; Steinkamp et al., 2020; Thiel et al., 2004; Weissman & Prado, 2012; gaze cues: Böckler et al., 2016; Caruana et al., 2015; Koike et al., 2019; Lee et al., 2010; Sato et al., 2016; Turk-Browne et al., 2013). In contrast, in other experiments, both social and non-social cues were used (e.g., Greene et al., 2009; Hietanen et al., 2006; Joseph et al., 2014; Redcay et al., 2010, 2012). While experiments with non-social cues, or with both arrow and gaze cues, involved similar tasks (i.e., detection tasks, e.g., Hietanen et al., 2006; Mao et al., 2007; Thiel et al., 2004; discrimination tasks, e.g., Dombert et al., 2016; Greene et al., 2009; Joseph et al., 2014; Natale et al., 2009; Noesselt et al., 2002; Peelen et al., 2004; Small et al., 2003; Steinkamp et al., 2020; Weissman & Prado, 2012), experiments with social cues could either involve detection or discrimination tasks (e.g., Böckler et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2010; Sato et al., 2016; Turk-Browne et al., 2013), or joint attention tasks (e.g., Caruana et al., 2015; Koike et al., 2019, 2010; Redcay et al., 2012). In this latter case, to allow comparisons with the typical gaze cueing tasks, only joint attention studies that required detection or discrimination of targets presented at the cued/uncued location were included. Finally, the included experiments differed in terms of cue predictive validity as cues could be predictive (e.g., Caruana et al., 2015; Dombert et al., 2016; Natale et al., 2009; Redcay et al., 2010, 2012; Small et al., 2003; Steinkamp et al., 2020; Thiel et al., 2004; Turk-Browne et al., 2013; Weissman & Prado, 2012), or not (i.e., Böckler et al., 2016; Greene et al., 2009; Hietanen et al., 2006; Joseph et al., 2014; Koike et al., 2019; Lee et al., 2010; Mao et al., 2007; Noesselt et al., 2002; Peelen et al., 2004; Sato et al., 2016) of the target location. Importantly, this inclusion strategy is based on Müller et al. (2018), who recommends that a good meta-analysis might include different paradigms or tasks to investigate effects that are consistent across strategies, provided that all paradigms or tasks rely on similar higher-order supervisory control processes. Multiple experiments from the same subject group were handled according to the Turkeltaub Non-Additive procedure (HBM, 2012) to minimize the within-group effects. This together with using cluster-level thresholding allows controlling for the excessive contribution of one experiment (Eickhoff et al., 2016). For recent studies using a similar procedure see Sulpizio et al. (2020) and Langner et al. (2018).
Activation Likelihood Estimation
Activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis allows for assessing whether the clustering of activation foci from different experiments that tap the same cognitive function is significantly higher than that expected under the null distribution of a casual spatial association of results from the same experiments. ALE allows analysing the probability that a voxel contains at least one of the activation foci, producing a map that reflects the union of activation probabilities for each voxel. Thus, ALE assesses the overlap between foci and models the probability distributions centred on each one of them (Eickhoff et al., 2009). Using this method, we performed two separate ALE analyses on two categories of studies in relation to the type of cue (gaze vs. arrow). One investigator (CS) classified the experiments according to these categories and a second investigator (MB) double-checked the categorizations. When necessary, the three investigators discussed and resolved eventual classification doubts. After carrying out separate ALE analyses on the categories of studies, we performed a contrast analysis to directly compare the effects of the cues [(arrow > gaze) and (gaze > arrow)]. This contrast analysis allowed highlighting voxels, whose signal was greater in the first than in the second condition. We also carried out a conjunction analysis between two types of cueing [arrow ∧ gaze] to identify voxels that subtended both arrow- and gaze-cueing.
The ALE meta-analysis was performed using GingerALE 3.0.2 (brainmap.org), with MNI coordinates (Talairach coordinates were converted automatically into MNI coordinates by using GingerALE). Here we used the non-additive procedure proposed by Turkeltaub et al. (HBM, 2012) to minimize within-experiment effects. According to Eickhoff et al.’s (2009) modified procedure, the ALE values of each voxel in the brain were computed, and the null distribution of the ALE statistic was calculated for each voxel. The Full-Width Half-Maximum (FWHM) value was automatically computed because this parameter is empirically determined (Eickhoff et al., 2009). The thresholded ALE maps for the two separate ALE analyses were computed using p values from the previous step, a cluster-level inference at the 0.05 level of significance and a cluster forming threshold at p < 0.001 (uncorrected), with 1000 permutations (Eickhoff et al., 2016). The thresholded ALE maps for the contrast and conjunction analyses were computed using 10,000 permutations and a thresholding value of 0.05, with a minimum cluster size of 200 mm3.
Results
Brain Network of Gaze Cueing
The ALE analysis on gaze cueing highlighted a cluster of activation in the middle temporal gyrus, extending towards the superior temporal gyrus and temporoparietal junction (TPJ) in the right hemisphere. This cluster encompassed the posterior portion of the superior temporal sulcus (STS). In the right hemisphere, we found activation in the precentral gyrus (PcG), extending to the middle frontal gyrus (MFG), likely corresponding to the territory of the human frontal eye fields (FEF; Amiez & Petrides, 2009)Footnote 1. We also found a cluster of activation in the right inferior parietal lobule (IPL), extending to the angular gyrus (AG). Results are shown in Tables 2 and Fig. 2 (red-to-yellow patches).
Brain Network of Arrow Cueing
The individual ALE analysis on arrow cueing revealed bilateral clusters of activation in the bilateral precuneus, extending towards the superior parietal lobule in the left hemisphere. We also found cluster of activation in the left inferior parietal lobule (IPL), mainly located in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS). The right superior frontal gyrus, in the territory of the human FEFFootnote 2, was consistently activated for arrow cueing, as well. Results are shown in Tables 3 and Fig. 2 (blue-to-light green patches).
Conjunction Analysis
The conjunction analysis revealedno suprathreshold clusters of activation.
Contrast Analyses
Gaze vs. Arrow Cueing
This contrast showed no suprathreshold clusters of activation.
Arrow vs. Gaze Cueing
The direct comparison between arrow cueing and gaze cueing showed clusters of voxels spanning from the precuneus to the superior parietal lobe in the left hemisphere; these regions showed stronger convergence for experiments using arrow cueing as compared to those using gaze cueing. Results are shown in Table 4; Fig. 3.
Discussion
The present ALE-meta-analysis was aimed at assessing the brain areas activated by gaze and arrow cues to shed some light on the unresolved question of whether orienting attention by social, gaze and non-social, arrow cues differ regarding the neural mechanisms involved. To date, there are two competing hypotheses: one posing that the attentional mechanisms triggered by gaze cues are different from those involved by non-social cues such as arrows, and consequently orienting by these two types of cues should rely on different neural underpinnings. Alternatively, the same attentional mechanism is triggered by gaze and arrow cues, and therefore orienting based on these cues relies on the same neural circuits. We compared the clustering of activation foci from 40 single experiments, involving a total number of 806 participants, from the 20 selected papers, assessed the overlap between foci, and modelled the probability distributions centred on each one of them (Eickhoff et al., 2009). Our findings support the claim that orienting of attention by social gaze and by non-social arrow cues are at least in part functionally segregated. Findings of the conjunction analysis did not reveal any suprathreshold clusters of activation and only social gaze cues activate brain areas involved in processing variant aspects of faces and in mental state attribution. This is in keeping with the proposal that the neural mechanisms involved in shifting attention based on social cues also rely on those involved in mental state attribution (see Calder et al., 2002; Senju et al., 2004), and with evidence showing that children orient their attention based on gaze cues earlier than they do based on arrow cues (e.g., Jakobsen et al., 2013).
More specifically, our findings show that the cluster of activation for gaze cues encompassed the posterior portion of the Superior Temporal Sulcus (STS), underlying processing variant aspects of faces such as gaze, facial expression, and lip movements (Engell & Haxby, 2007), suggesting the involvement of the temporoparietal junction, linked to mental state attribution (e.g., Schurz et al., 2017). The anterior STS is associated with gaze perception and the posterior STS with expectancy violations, such as those elicited by the incongruence between gaze direction and a salient target (e.g., Vander Wyk et al., 2009). This cluster of activity is compatible with the proposal that the temporoparietal junction reflects a link between the STS and lateral parietal regions, which mediates gaze-cued attentional shifts (i.e., Carlin & Calder, 2013). Importantly, this area is not present in the cluster of activation for arrow cues, which in turn shows bilateral frontoparietal activation, encompassing the superior frontal gyrus, precuneus, and the superior parietal lobule. The connections between the precuneus and the superior frontal gyrus have been linked to dorsal attention and frontoparietal control networks (Luo et al., 2019). In contrast, for gaze cues we found a cluster of activation in the right IPL, pointing towards a role of this region in the endogenous orienting and maintenance of attention to a target location. For both types of cues, there was a pattern of activation encompassing the territory of human frontal eye fields (FEF). Although, when contrasting the pattern of activation observed for the gaze and arrow cues, only arrow cues were associated with stronger activity in the area spanning from the precuneus to the superior parietal lobe in the left hemisphere. To our knowledge, the contribution of the FEF to social attention is not well documented and the present findings point to the FEF being involved in both social and non-social orienting (e.g., Torriero et al., 2019). Therefore, the present findings show that brain regions involved in endogenous attention – in the territory of the FEF in the superior frontal gyrus – are consistently activated by both gaze and arrow cueing. This is not surprising, as the FEF cluster of activity reflects overt attentional shifts to the cued location linked to eye movements (e.g., Schlag-Rey et al., 1997), whereas the activity of the FEF and the IPS has been linked to sustained maintenance of attention at peripheral locations (e.g., Kelley et al., 2007). However, although both arrow- and gaze-cues activated the FEF, the activation for arrow cues is more toward the superior FEF whereas that for gaze cues is toward the middle FEF. The function of this fine-grained localization is for future research to clarify. Importantly, the cluster of activation for gaze cues was lateralized to the right hemisphere is in keeping with lateralization for face processing (see Rossion & Lochy, 2022 for a recent review) and emotion-attention lateralization (e.g., De Luca et al., 2020; Pecchinenda et al., 2021; see also Hartikainen, 2021 for a recent review).
The present findings also show the activity of the right middle frontal gyrus for gaze-cues, and this area is one of the core regions of the ventral attention network that also includes the temporoparietal junction. The activity of the right middle frontal gyrus reflects reorienting attention, as it is the convergence site of the dorsal and ventral attention networks (e.g., Corbetta et al., 2008), and it is seen as a circuit-breaker to interrupt ongoing endogenous attentional processes in the dorsal network and reorient attention to an exogenous stimulus (e.g., Doricchi et al., 2010). Therefore, the right middle frontal gyrus would exert control over the dorsal and the ventral attention networks, for the flexible modulation of endogenous and exogenous attention. Within the network activation to targets, which is greater with spatial unpredictability, our findings for gaze cues point to a pivotal role of the right temporoparietal junction and the cuneus indicating engagement by stimulus-driven orienting, including activation due to target appearance at one spatial location. This evidence has been interpreted as showing two largely dissociated neural networks mediating endogenous and exogenous/stimulus-driven control of visuospatial selective attention (for a review, see Geng & Vossel, 2013), although the specific contribution of the IPS to endogenous or exogenous orienting of attention is still a matter of debate (e.g., Vandenberghe et al., 2005; Serences et al., 2005; Corbetta et al., 2008; Vandenberghe & Gillebert, 2009).
Some limitations of the current meta-analysis should be acknowledged. Firstly, the necessity to collapse valid and invalid trial data of different studies. Although this allows to achieve good power to reliably detect common areas activated during orienting of attention to social and non-social cues and it is in keeping with current recommendations (Müller et al. 2018), it precludes disentangling the neural mechanism underlying the advantages in shifting attention on valid trials from those underlying the costs of shifting attention on invalid trials for social and non-social cues. Secondly, although the method used in the current meta-analysis avoids capitalization on chance (e.g., cherry-picking the voxels showing the highest correlations), circumventing the potential risk of bias also relies on the goodness of the individual studies included in the meta-analysis. In fact, the risk of publication bias is still an open issue (see Müller et al. 2018). Moreover, coordinate-based algorithms are insensitive to non-significant results and the publication bias may go unnoticed. This is because coordinate-based neuroimaging meta-analyses are conceptually different from conventional effect-size meta-analyses as they test spatial convergence of effects across experiments with the null-hypothesis, which assumes random spatial convergence. We believe that the current results are pivotal in contributing to future research by pointing to new regions of interest into which theoretically sound investigations may be performed and by guiding fMRI studies to conduct hypothesis-driven analyses. Therefore, the current meta-analysis would have the potential impact of contributing to reducing cherry-picking. Moreover, it should also be noted that gaze and arrow cues may call upon different attentional selection mechanisms– object-based attention for gaze cueing and location-based attention for arrow cueing (i.e., Marotta et al., 2012; for more recent evidence, see Chacón-Candia et al., 2020). However, the current ALE-meta-analysis does not allow to disentangle between them. This would require using specific experimental manipulations, which may not be suitable in conjunction with fMRIs, but could be used in combination with Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation techniques. Finally, it would be interesting to extend the findings of the present meta-analysis by including experimental characteristics of the individual studies as moderators, but this requires more studies using similar methodologies.
Conclusions
In conclusion, findings from the present ALE-meta-analysis show that orienting attention by gaze and arrow cues relies on the activation of brain regions involved in endogenous attention (e.g., the FEF in the region of the superior frontal gyrus), whereas brain regions involved in exogenous attention and mental state attribution (e.g., temporoparietal junction, middle frontal gyrus) are activated only by gaze cues. This finding points toward gaze- and arrow cues – two perceptually asymmetrical stimuli, one social and the other non-social – being overlearned signals that yield efficient attentional orienting, which is subserved by partially segregated brain networks. Albeit the present findings are correlational, they could guide future research using Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation to directly assess the contribution of brain areas involved in endogenous and exogenous attention in orienting attention by social and non-social, directional cues. Overall, the present results provide a theoretically motivated network that future neuroimaging studies, especially fMRI studies, may use to frame their analyses, in a hypothesis-driven fashion (Turkeltaub et al., 2002; Poldrack, 2007).
Data Availability
The author confirms that all data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article. The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author [AP].
Notes
Posterior probability of the term “eye fields” to be used in a study if activation is observed at x = 38, y = 2, z = 48 is 0.85 (data from neurosynth.org; Yarkoni et al., 2011).
Posterior probability of the term “eye fields” to be used in a study if activation is observed at x = 24, y = 6, z = 56 is 0.80 (data from neurosynth.org; Yarkoni et al., 2011).
References
Amiez, C., & Petrides, M. (2009). Anatomical organization of the eye fields in the human and non-human primate frontal cortex. Progress in Neurobiology, 89(2), 220–230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.07.010
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press.
Böckler, A., Eskenazi, T., Sebanz, N., & Rueschemeyer, S. A. (2016). (How) observed eye-contact modulates gaze following. An fMRI study. Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(1–4), 55–66. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2015.1053442
Brignani, D., Guzzon, D., Marzi, C. A., & Miniussi, C. (2009). Attentional orienting induced by arrows and eye-gaze compared with an endogenous cue. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 370–381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.011
Calder, A. J., Lawrence, A. D., Keane, J., Scott, S. K., Owen, A. M., Christoffels, I., & Young, A. W. (2002). Reading the mind from eye gaze. Neuropsychologia, 40(8), 1129–1138. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00008-8
Carlin, J. D., & Calder, A. J. (2013). The neural basis of eye gaze processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(3), 450–455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.014
Caruana, N., Brock, J., & Woolgar, A. (2015). A frontotemporoparietal network common to initiating and responding to joint attention bids. NeuroImage, 108, 34–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.041
Chacón-Candia, J. A., Lupiáñez, J., Casagrande, M., & Marotta, A. (2020). Sex differences in attentional selection following Gaze and Arrow Cues. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00095
Chica, A. B., Martín-Arévalo, E., Botta, F., & Lupiáñez, J. (2014). The spatial orienting paradigm: How to design and interpret spatial attention experiments. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 40, 35–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.002
Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755
Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017
Dalmaso, M., Castelli, L., & Galfano, G. (2020). Social modulators of gaze-mediated orienting of attention: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(5), 833–855. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01730-x
De Luca, F., Petrucci, M., Monachesi, B., Lavidor, M., & Pecchinenda, A. (2020). Asymmetric contributions of the fronto-parietal network to emotional conflict in the word–face interference task. Symmetry 12(10), 1701. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12101701
Dodd, M. D., & Wilson, D. (2009). Training attention: Interactions between central cues and reflexive attention. Visual Cognition, 17(5), 736–754. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280802340711
Dombert, P. L., Kuhns, A., Mengotti, P., Fink, G. R., & Vossel, S. (2016). Functional mechanisms of probabilistic inference in feature- and space-based attentional systems. NeuroImage, 142, 553–564. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.010
Doricchi, F., Macci, E., Silvetti, M., & Macaluso, E. (2010). Neural correlates of the spatial and expectancy components of endogenous and stimulus-driven orienting of attention in the Posner task. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1574–1585. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp215
Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6(5), 509–540. https://doi.org/10.1080/135062899394920
Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Grefkes, C., Wang, L. E., Zilles, K., & Fox, P. T. (2009). Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: A random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Human Brain Mapping, 30(9), 2907–2926. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20718
Eickhoff, S. B., Nichols, T. E., Laird, A. R., Hoffstaedter, F., Amunts, K., Fox, P. T., & Eickhoff, C. R. (2016). Behavior, sensitivity, and power of activation likelihood estimation characterized by massive empirical simulation. NeuroImage, 137, 70–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.072
Engell, A. D., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). Facial expression and gaze-direction in human superior temporal sulcus. Neuropsychologia, 45(14), 3234–3241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.022
Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(27), 9673–9678. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504136102
Fox, M. D., Corbetta, M., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2006). Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(26), 10046–10051. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0604187103
Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(3), 490–495. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03208827
Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2001). The Biological Basis of Social Interaction. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(5), 151–155. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00137
Funes, M. J., Lupiáñez, J., & Milliken, B. (2007). Separate mechanisms recruited by exogenous and endogenous spatial cues: Evidence from a spatial Stroop paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(2), 348–362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.348
Geng, J. J., & Vossel, S. (2013). Re-evaluating the role of TPJ in attentional control: Contextual updating? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10), 2608–2620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.010
Greene, D. J., Mooshagian, E., Kaplan, J. T., Zaidel, E., & Iacoboni, M. (2009). The neural correlates of social attention: Automatic orienting to social and nonsocial cues. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 73(4), 499–511. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0233-3
Hartikainen, K. M. (2021). Emotion-attention Interaction in the right hemisphere. Brain Sciences, 11, 1006. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11081006
Hietanen, J. K., Nummenmaa, L., Nyman, M. J., Parkkola, R., & Hämäläinen, H. (2006). Automatic attention orienting by social and symbolic cues activates different neural networks: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 33(1), 406–413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.048
Hietanen, J. K., Leppänen, J. M., Nummenmaa, L., & Astikainen, P. (2008). Visuospatial attention shifts by gaze and arrow cues: An ERP study. Brain Research, 1215, 123–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.03.091
Hommel, B., Pratt, J., Colzato, L., & Godijn, R. (2001). Symbolic control of visual attention. Psychological Science, 12(5), 360–365. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00367
Jakobsen, K. V., Frick, J. E., & Simpson, E. A. (2013). Look Here! The development of attentional orienting to symbolic cues. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(2), 229–249. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.666772
Joseph, R. M., Fricker, Z., & Keehn, B. (2014). Activation of frontoparietal attention networks by non-predictive gaze and arrow cues. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(2), 294–301. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu054
Kelley, T. A., Serences, J. T., Giesbrecht, B., & Yantis, S. (2007). Cortical mechanisms for shifting and holding Visuospatial attention. Cerebral Cortex, 18(1), 114–125. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm036
Koike, T., Tanabe, H. C., Adachi-Abe, S., Okazaki, S., Nakagawa, E., Sasaki, A. T., & Sadato, N. (2019). Role of the right anterior insular cortex in joint attention-related identification with a partner. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(10), 1131–1145. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsz087
Langner, R., Leiberg, S., Hoffstaedter, F., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2018). Towards a human self-regulation system: Common and distinct neural signatures of emotional and behavioural control. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 90, 400–410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.022
Langton, S. R. H., & Bruce, V. (1999). Reflexive visual orienting in response to the Social attention of others. Visual Cognition, 6(5), 541–567. https://doi.org/10.1080/135062899394939
Laufs, H., Krakow, K., Sterzer, P., Eger, E., Beyerle, A., Salek-Haddadi, A., & Kleinschmidt, A. (2003). Electroencephalographic signatures of attentional and cognitive default modes in spontaneous brain activity fluctuations at rest. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(19), 11053–11058. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1831638100
Lee, L. C., Andrews, T. J., Johnson, S. J., Woods, W., Gouws, A., Green, G. G. R., & Young, A. W. (2010). Neural responses to rigidly moving faces displaying shifts in social attention investigated with fMRI and MEG. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 477–490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.005
Luo, Z., Zeng, L. L., Qin, J., Hou, C., Shen, H., & Hu, D. (2019). Functional parcellation of human brain Precuneus using density-based clustering. Cerebral Cortex, 30(1), 269–282. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz086
Mao, L., Zhou, B., Zhou, W., & Han, S. (2007). Neural correlates of covert orienting of visual spatial attention along vertical and horizontal dimensions. Brain Research, 1136, 142–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.031
Marotta, A., Lupiáñez, J., Martella, D., & Casagrande, M. (2012). Eye gaze versus arrows as spatial cues: Two qualitatively different modes of attentional selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(2), 326–335. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023959
McKay, K. T., Grainger, S. A., Coundouris, S. P., Skorich, D. P., Phillips, L. H., & Henry, J. D. (2021). Visual attentional orienting by eye gaze: A meta-analytic review of the gaze-cueing effect. Psychological Bulletin, 147(12), 1269–1289. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000353
Müller, V. I., Cieslik, E. C., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., Tench, C. R., Yarkoni, T., Nichols, T. E., Turkeltaub, P. E., Wager, T. V., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2018). Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 84, 151–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.012
Natale, E., Marzi, C. A., & Macaluso, E. (2009). FMRI correlates of visuo-spatial reorienting investigated with an attention shifting double-cue paradigm. Human Brain Mapping, 30(8), 2367–2381. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20675
Noesselt, T., Hillyard, S. A., Woldorff, M. G., Schoenfeld, A., Hagner, T., Jäncke, L., & Heinze, H. J. (2002). Delayed striate cortical activation during spatial attention. Neuron, 35(3), 575–587. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00781-x
Oberwelland, E., Schilbach, L., Barisic, I., Krall, S. C., Vogeley, K., Fink, G. R., & Schulte-Rüther, M. (2016). Look into my eyes: Investigating joint attention using interactive eye-tracking and fMRI in a developmental sample. NeuroImage, 130, 248–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.026
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Plos Medicine, 18(3), e1003583. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1003583
Pecchinenda, A., & Petrucci, M. (2021). Emotion first: Children prioritize emotional faces in gaze-cued attentional orienting. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 85, 101–111. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01237-8
Pecchinenda, A., De Luca, F., Monachesi, B., Petrucci, M., Pazzaglia, M., Doricchi, F., & Lavidor, M. (2021). Contributions of the Right Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to the Attentional Blink: A tDCS Study Symmetry, 13(7), 1208. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym13071208
Peelen, M. V., Heslenfeld, D. J., & Theeuwes, J. (2004). Endogenous and exogenous attention shifts are mediated by the same large-scale neural network. NeuroImage, 22(2), 822–830. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.044
Poldrack, R. A. (2007). Region of interest analysis for fMRI. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(1), 67–70. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm006
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231
Redcay, E., Dodell-Feder, D., Pearrow, M. J., Mavros, P. L., Kleiner, M., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Saxe, R. (2010). Live face-to-face interaction during fMRI: A new tool for social cognitive neuroscience. NeuroImage, 50(4), 1639–1647. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.052
Redcay, E., Kleiner, M., & Saxe, R. (2012). Look at this: The neural correlates of initiating and responding to bids for joint attention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00169
Ristic, J., Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (2002). Are eyes special? It depends on how you look at it. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 507–513. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196306
Rossion, B., & Lochy, A. (2022). Is human face recognition lateralized to the right hemisphere due to neural competition with left-lateralized visual word recognition? A critical review. Brain Structure and Function, 227, 599–629. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-021-02370-0
Saito, D. N., Tanabe, H. C., Izuma, K., Hayashi, M. J., Morito, Y., Komeda, H., & Sadato, N. (2010). Stay Tuned”: Inter-individual neural synchronization during mutual gaze and joint attention. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2010.00127
Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Uono, S., & Toichi, M. (2016). Neural mechanisms underlying conscious and unconscious attentional shifts triggered by eye gaze. NeuroImage, 124, 118–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.061
Schlag-Rey, M., Amador, N., Sanchez, H., & Schlag, J. (1997). Antisaccade performance predicted by neuronal activity in the supplementary eye field. Nature, 390(6658), 398–401. https://doi.org/10.1038/37114
Schurz, M., Tholen, M. G., Perner, J., Mars, R. B., & Sallet, J. (2017). Specifying the brain anatomy underlying temporo-parietal junction activations for theory of mind: A review using probabilistic atlases from different imaging modalities. Human Brain Mapping, 38(9), 4788–4805. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23675
Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Dairoku, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2004). Reflexive orienting in response to eye gaze and an arrow in children with and without autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 445–458. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00236.x
Serences, J. T., Shomstein, S., Leber, A. B., Golay, X., Egeth, H. E., & Yantis, S. (2005). Coordination of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional control in human cortex. Psychological Science, 16, 114–122. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00791.x
Small, D., Gitelman, D., Gregory, M., Nobre, A., Parrish, T., & Mesulam, M. M. (2003). The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention. NeuroImage, 18(3), 633–641. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00012-5
Steinkamp, S. R., Vossel, S., Fink, G. R., & Weidner, R. (2020). Attentional reorientation along the meridians of the visual field: Are there different neural mechanisms at play? Human brain mapping, 41(13), 3765–3780. https://doi.org/10.1101/816165
Sulpizio, S., Del Maschio, N., Fedeli, D., & Abutalebi, J. (2020). Bilingual language processing: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 108, 834–853. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.12.014
Thiel, C. M., Zilles, K., & Fink, G. R. (2004). Cerebral correlates of alerting, orienting and reorienting of visuospatial attention: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 21(1), 318–328. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.044
Tipper, C. M., Handy, T. C., Giesbrecht, B., & Kingstone, A. (2008). Brain responses to Biological Relevance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(5), 879–891. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20510
Tipples, J. (2002). Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 314–318. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196287
Torriero, S., Mattavelli, G., Lo Gerfo, E., Romero Lauro, L., Actis-Grosso, R., & Ricciardelli, P. (2019). FEF Excitability in Attentional Bias: A TMS-EEG study. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience, 12, 333. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00333
Turk-Browne, N. B., Golomb, J. D., & Chun, M. M. (2013). Complementary attentional components of successful memory encoding. NeuroImage, 66, 553–562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.053
Turkeltaub, P. E., Eden, G. F., Jones, K. M., & Zeffiro, T. A. (2002). Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading. :method and validation Neuroimage, 16, 765–780.
Turkeltaub, P. E., Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Fox, M., Wiener, M., & Fox, P. (2012). Minimizing within-experiment and within‐group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta‐analyses. Human Brain Mapping, 33(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21186
Vandenberghe, R., & Gillebert, C. R. (2009). Parcellation of parietal cortex: Convergence between lesion-symptom mapping and mapping of the intact functioning brain. Behavioural Brain Research, 199, 171–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.12.005
Vandenberghe, R., Geeraerts, S., Molenberghs, P., Lafosse, C., Vandenbulcke, M., Peeters, K., Peeters, R., Van Hecke, P., & Orban, G. A. (2005). Attentional responses to unattended stimuli in human parietal cortex. Brain, 128, 2843–2857. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh522
Vander Wyk, B. C., Hudac, C. M., Carter, E. J., Sobel, D. M., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2009). Action understanding in the Superior temporal Sulcus Region. Psychological Science, 20(6), 771–777. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02359.x
Vincent, J. L., Snyder, A. Z., Fox, M. D., Shannon, B. J., Andrews, J. R., Raichle, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2006). Coherent spontaneous activity identifies a hippocampal-parietal Memory Network. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96(6), 3517–3531. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00048.2006
Weissman, D. H., & Prado, J. (2012). Heightened activity in a key region of the ventral attention network is linked to reduced activity in a key region of the dorsal attention network during unexpected shifts of covert visual spatial attention. NeuroImage, 61(4), 798–804. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.032
Yarkoni, T., Poldrack, R. A., Nichols, T. E., Van Essen, D. C., & Wager, T. D. (2011). Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nature Methods, 8(8), 665–670. https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.1635
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Francesca De Luca for help in extracting fMRI coordinates in an earlier phase of the project.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Università degli Studi di Roma La Sapienza within the CRUI-CARE Agreement. We acknowledge the following funding: Claudia Salera is funded by a PhD studentship in Behavioural Neuroscience from the Ministero dell’Università e della Ricerca (M.U.R). Anna Pecchinenda is funded by the Ministero dell’Università e della Ricerca (M.U.R.), grant number: RM120172B77EE5F8 and by an EPS grant from the Experimental Psychology Society (U.K.).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Anna Pecchinenda and Maddalena Boccia contributed to the study conception and design. Analyses were performed by Maddalena Boccia. Material preparation and data collection were performed by Claudia Salera and Maddalena Boccia. The first draft of the manuscript was written by Anna Pecchinenda and Maddalena Boccia and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Registration and Protocol
The present review was not registered and a protocol was not prepared.
Ethics
This is a systematic review and meta-analysis of published data and no ethical approval is required.
Competing Interests
The authors have no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Highlights
• Patterns of brain activation in orienting attention by social and non-social cues.
• Superior frontal gyrus and frontal eye field show activation for both cues.
• Gaze and arrow cues activate partially segregated brain networks.
• Only gaze cues engage brain areas involved in mental state attribution.
Appendix 1
Appendix 1
Table 5
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Salera, C., Boccia, M. & Pecchinenda, A. Segregation of Neural Circuits Involved in Social Gaze and Non-Social Arrow Cues: Evidence from an Activation Likelihood Estimation Meta-Analysis. Neuropsychol Rev 34, 496–510 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-023-09593-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-023-09593-4