Skip to main content
Log in

Anxiety and defensiveness: individual differences in affective startle modulation

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Anxious individuals selectively attend to threatening information, and defensiveness may influence the experience and expression of anxiety. Fifty-eight undergraduates scoring high and low on measures of anxiety and defensiveness viewed pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant pictures. Acoustic startle probes were presented 60, 240, or 2,000 ms after picture onset. At 60 ms and 240 ms, repressors (low anxiety, high defensiveness) showed weaker blinks during both pleasant and unpleasant compared to neutral pictures, suggesting enhanced early attention to affective stimuli, regardless of valence. At 2,000 ms, high-anxious but not low anxious participants showed potentiated startle responses during unpleasant compared to pleasant pictures. Although this result replicated previous research on anxiety and valence modulation of startle, exploratory analyses suggested that the valence effect was restricted to trait anxious individuals tending toward a defensive coping style. Across probe conditions, defensiveness was associated with heightened startle reactivity independent of self-reported anxiety and foreground stimulus characteristics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. IAPS slide numbers: Unpleasant, males: 2480, 2570, 3015, 3071, 3102, 3110, 3120, 3130, 3140, 3168, 3170, 3180, 3266, 3350, 3530, 7185, 7187, 9040, 9405, 9410, 9520, 9560, 9570, 9810; females: 2800, 3015, 3030, 3102, 3110, 3120, 3130, 3140, 3168, 3170, 3180, 3266, 3301, 3530, 6313, 9040, 9253, 9405, 9410, 9433, 9570, 9571, 9800, 9921. Pleasant slides, males: 1710, 4002, 4150, 4180, 4210, 4220, 4232, 4250, 4607, 4652, 4659, 4664, 4670, 4680, 5130, 5510, 5700, 7000, 7090, 8080, 8170, 8190, 8501; females: 1463, 1710, 1811, 2050, 2058, 2150, 2160, 2165, 2209, 2216, 2550, 4610, 5621, 5910, 7270, 7330, 7502, 8190, 8200, 8370, 8380, 8420, 8470, 8496; Neutral slides, males and females: 2190, 2440, 2840, 3030, 5520, 5530, 5740, 6570, 7006, 7020, 7025, 7035, 7040, 7050, 7060, 7150, 7217, 7233, 7235, 7490, 7491, 7950, 9433, 9910. Ratings of individual pictures are available from the authors upon request.

References

  • Aitken, C. J., Siddle, D. A., & Lipp, O. V. (1999). The effects of threat and nonthreat word lead stimuli on blink modification. Psychophysiology, 36, 699-705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, B. J., & Graham, F. K. (1985). Blink reflex modification by selective attention: Evidence for the modulation of “automatic” processing. Biological Psychology, 21, 43-59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balaban, M., Losito, B., Simons, R. F., & Graham, F. K. (1986). Off-line latency and amplitude scoring of the human reflex eyeblink with Fortran IV. Psychophysiology, 23, 612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Pictures as prepulse: Attention and emotion in startle modification. Psychophysiology, 30, 541-545.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosschot, J. F., de Ruiter, C., & Kindt, M. (1999). Recall and recognition of threatening, pleasant, and neutral words in repressors. European Journal of Personality, 13, 1-14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, L. L., Tomarken, A. J., Orth, D. N., Loosen, P. T., Kalin, N. H., & Davidson, R. J. (1996). Individual differences in repressive-defensiveness predict basal salivary cortisol levels. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 362-371.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo, M.G., & Eysenck, M.W. (2000). Early vigilance and late avoidance of threat processing: Repressive coping versus low/high anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 14, 763–787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, E. W. III (1999). Affective individual differences, psychopathology, and startle reflex modulation. In M. E. Dawson, A. M. Schell, & A. H. Bohmelt (Eds.), Startle modification: Implications for neuroscience, cognitive science, and clinical science (pp. 187–208). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, E. W. III, Atkinson, L., & Lang, K. G. (1987). Stimulus control and data acquisition for IBM PC’s and compatibles. Psychophysiology, 24, 726-727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, E. W. III, & Hawk, L. W. (1998). A repeated measures regression approach to psychophysiological data analysis. Psychophysiology, 35, S32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, E. W. III, Hawk, L. W., Davis, T. L., & Stevenson, V. E. (1991). Affective individual differences and startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 5-13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crowne, D. P., & Marlow, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349-354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, K., & Furnham, A. (1989). The colour naming of emotional words. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 383-389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (1998). Working memory capacity in high trait-anxious and repressor groups. Cognition & Emotion, 12, 697-713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (1999). Are repressors self-deceivers or other-deceivers? Cognition & Emotion, 13, 1-17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (2001). Manipulation of focus of attention and its effects in anxiety in high trait-anxious individuals and repressors. Anxiety, Stress and Coping: An International Journal, 14, 173-191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W. (1997). Anxiety and cognition: A unified theory. Hove, England: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (1993). Allocation of visual attention and anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 7, 207-215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (1996). Selective processing of threatening words in anxiety: The role of awareness. Cognition & Emotion, 10, 449-480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fridlund, A. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567-589.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Globisch, J., Hamm, A.O., Esteves, F., & Öhman, A. (1999). Fear appears fast: Temporal course of startle reflex potentiation in animal fearful subjects. Psychophysiology, 36, 66–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hawk, L. W., Stevenson, V. E., & Cook, E. W. III (1992). The effects of eyelid closure on affective imagery and eyeblink startle. Journal of Psychophysiology, 6, 299-310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hock, M., & Krohne, H. W. (2004). Coping with threat and memory for ambiguous information: Testing the repressive discontinuity hypothesis. Emotion, 4, 65–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jorgensen, R. S., Johnson, B. T., Kolodziej, M. E., & Schreer, G. E. (1996). Elevated blood pressure and personality: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 293–320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, J. P., Schwartz, G. E., Allen, J. J., & Dikman, Z. V. (1998). Perceptual and electroencephalographic registration of masked emotional words in defensiveness: An exploratory study. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 499–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Öhman, A., & Vaitl, D. (1999). The international affective picture system [photographic slides]. Gainesville, FL: Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaRowe, S. D., Kline, J. P., & Patrick C. J. (2004). Defensiveness is related to increased startle magnitude. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1441–1451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, D. K., Norton, G. R., Walker, J. R., & Stein, M. B. (2002). Analysis of startle responses in patients with panic disorder and social phobia. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 31, 156–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levenston, G. K., Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). The psychopath as observer: Emotion and attention in picture processing. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 373–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lorch , R. F. Jr., & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analysis of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 149–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., & Rutherford, E. M. (1992). Anxiety and the selective processing of emotional information: Mediating roles of awareness, trait and state variables, and personal relevance of stimulus materials. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 479–491.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 16, 331–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S. (1992). Evolutionary memories, emotional processing, and the emotional disorders. In D. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 161–206). Orlando, FL: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews, A. (1993). Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 304–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, L. B. (1998). Repressive coping, trait anxiety, and reported avoidance of negative thoughts. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 299–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newton, T. L., & Contrada, R. J. (1992). Repressive coping and verbal autonomic response dissociation: The influence of social context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 159–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., MacCallum, R. C., & Nicewander, W. A. (2005). Use of the extreme groups approach: A critical reexamination and new recommendations. Psychological Methods, 10, 178–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. J. (1969). The conditions and consequences of evaluation apprehension. In R. Rosenthal & R. L. Rosnow (Eds.), Artifact in behavioral research (pp. 143–179). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. D. (1970). STAI: Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R. D., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, C. B., Hall, J. A., Miller, F. D., Dewhirst, J. R., Fine, G. A., Taylor, M., & Rosnow, R. L. (1979). Evaluation apprehension, social desirability, and the interpretation of test correlations. Social Behavioral and Personality, 7, 193–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanman, E. J., Boehmelt, A. H., Dawson, M. E., & Schell, A. M. (1996). The varying time course of attentional and affective modulation of the startle eyeblink reflex. Psychophysiology, 33, 691–697.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volz, M., Hamm, A. O., Kirsch, P., & Rey, E. R. (2003). Temporal course of emotional startle modulation in schizophrenia patients. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 49, 123–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vrana, S. R., Spence, E. L., & Lang, P. J. (1988). The startle response: A new measure of emotion? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 487–491.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinberger, D. A., Schwartz, G. E., & Davidson, J. R. (1979). Low-anxious, high-anxious, and repressive coping styles: Psychometric patterns and behavioral and physiological responses to stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 369–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was completed as part of the first author’s doctoral dissertation under the supervision of the second author. The authors wish to acknowledge the following individuals who served on the dissertation committee, and who made valuable contributions to the study: Lori L. Davis, M.D., Sheryl R. Jackson, Ph.D., Roy C. Martin, Ph.D., Michael E. Sloane, Ph.D., and Rex A. Wright, Ph.D.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Richard O. Temple.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Temple, R.O., Cook, E.W. Anxiety and defensiveness: individual differences in affective startle modulation. Motiv Emot 31, 115–123 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9062-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9062-0

Keywords

Navigation