Skip to main content
Log in

What the Visual Word Recognition Skills of Prelingually Deafened Readers Tell About Their Reading Comprehension Problems

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities Aims and scope Submit manuscript

The objective of this study was to clarify the nature and efficiency of the word recognition skills of prelingually deafened Hebrew readers and the way these skills relate to their reading comprehension level. Relevant data were gathered by means of a research paradigm demanding the same/different categorization of phonologically or formationally manipulated word pairs and by a sentence comprehension test (SCT). Participants were prelingually deafened individuals (n=22, mean grade=8.7) who attested to using sign language as their preferred means for communication, and a task-matched hearing control group (n=40, mean grade=8.9). In general, findings suggest that both hearing readers and readers with prelingual deafness rely upon orthographic knowledge rather than upon their primary language for mediating the processing of written words in the working memory (WM). As predicted, in comparison to their hearing counterparts, the reading comprehension of the prelingually deafened participants, as a group, was significantly impaired. Comparative analyses of quantitative and qualitative aspects of the same/different categorizations of word pairs by the participant groups, and the way these correlated with performance on the SCT clearly indicated, however, that this weakness is not rooted in poor word recognition skills but probably reflects a lack of syntactic knowledge crucial for the adequate, post-lexical processing of recognized words. Finally, the findings convincingly show that, despite their profound hearing loss, some individuals who were prelingually deaf acquired rule-based knowledge for the adequate processing of written text.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Israeli sign language (ISL) is a full-fledged language used by the deaf community in Israel. As in ASL, its vocabulary is built systematically according to limited sets of formational parameters such as hand shape, hand movement, place of articulation, etc.

  2. The term ‘‘conventional identicalness’’ refers to an identity between two words that cannot be determined on a mere perceptual basis, but that must be mediated by some form of knowledge.

  3. The sentence comprehension test used for attaining the reading comprehension of the participants was an abbreviated, adjusted version of Miller’s syntactic ability test (SAT) (Miller, 2000).

  4. The similarity between Signed Hebrew and spoken Hebrew is limited to a rough matching of the sign order to the word order in Hebrew. There are, however, many incompatibilities between the two systems, such as an almost complete lack of devices in Signed Hebrew to represent the rather rich morphological structure of spoken Hebrew.

  5. Most alphabetic writing systems merge discrete letter-graphemes representing the consonants and vowels of words into visually distinct, serially ordered letter sequences. Unlike such writing systems, in Hebrew, a significant part of the vowel information is not represented by letter-graphemes inserted between consonantal letters, but by means of small diacritical marks (dashes and points), normally placed below, and clearly separated from, the consonantal letter string. However, diacritics in Hebrew orthography are optional and the average Hebrew reader is capable of reading a text even if they are not indicated. This form of Hebrew orthography is called unpointed Hebrew. Israeli reading materials above the early grade levels are printed almost exclusively in unpointed Hebrew. In this study, the Hebrew words used for assessing the word processing strategy of the participants and the words used for preparing the SAT were all unpointed.

  6. Hebrew is read from the right to the left.

  7. The relevant, basic, formational (linguistic) categories underlying the composition of a sign are hand shape, movement, and place of sign articulation relative to the body of the signer. Word pairs with formationally similar paralleling signs were built from two words that had paralleling signs that differed only on one of these formational parameters (e.g., same hand shape, same movement, different place of articulation; or, different hand shape, same movement, same place of articulation, etc.). In the control condition, the words paralleling signs differed on all of their formational parameters, except in two instances where the signs used the same place of articulation.

  8. The doubling of the stimuli pairs was necessary because rhyming words in which phonological congruity is not reflected at the graphemic level are very rare. The fact that such words had to be familiar to the participants with deafness restricted this already small word pool even further.

  9. All the words used for the preparation of the test sentences were taken from a word pool congregated by the author for the preparation of sentences in a previous experiment (Miller, 2000). All the words in this pool were rated by two teachers of the deaf and a speech therapist—all of them working closely with deaf children at the initial primary school levels—to be within the realm of the vocabulary of deaf second graders. Only words with existing paralleling sign were included in the word pool.

REFERENCES

  • American National Standards Institute (1989). Specifications for Audiometers (ANSI S3.6–1989), ANSI, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory, Clarendon Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellugi, U., Klima, E., and Siple, P. (1975). Remembering in signs. Cognition 3: 93–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonvillian, J. D. (1983). Effects of signability and imagery on word recall of deaf and hearing students. Percept. Motor Skills 56: 775–791.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charlier, B. L., and Leybaert, J. (2000). The rhyming skills of deaf children educated with phonetically augmented speech reading. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 53: 349–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, R. (1979). The Deaf School Child. Harper & Row, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dyer, A., MacSweeney, M., Szczerbinski, M., and Campbell, R. (2003). Predictors of reading delay in deaf adolescents: The relative contributions of rapid automatized naming speed and phonemic awareness and decoding. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 8: 215–229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S. A., and Willows, D. M. (2001). Systematic phonic instruction helps students learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 71: 393–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trials. Psychol. Bull. 123: 71–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamiltion, H., and Holzman, T. G. (1989). Linguistic coding in short-term memory as a function of stimulus type. Memory Cognition 17: 541–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, V. L. (1982). Short-term recall by deaf signers of American Sign Language: Implications of encoding strategy for order recall. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn., Memory, Cognition 8: 572–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, V. L. (1991). Phonological processing without sounds. In Brady, S., and Shankweiler, D. (eds.), Phonological Processes in Literacy: A Tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 153–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, V. L., and Fowler, C. A. (1987). Phonological coding in word reading: Evidence from hearing and deaf readers. Memory Cognition 15: 199–207.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, V. L., Liberman, I. Y., and Shankweiler, D. (1984). Linguistic coding in deaf children in relation to beginning reading success. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 37: 378–393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, V. L., and Lichtenstein, E. H. (1990). Short-term memory coding by deaf signers: The primary language coding hypothesis reconsidered. Cogn. Psychol. 22: 211–224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, V. L., and McGarr, N. S. (1989). Rhyme generation by deaf adults. J. Speech Hearing Res. 32: 2–11.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M., and Beech, J. R. (1998). Implicit phonological awareness and early reading development in prelingually deaf children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 3: 205–216.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M., and Moreno, C. (2004). Deaf children’s use of phonological coding: Evidence from reading, spelling, and working memory. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 9: 253–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izzo, A. (2002). Phonemic awareness and reading ability: An investigation with young readers who are deaf. Am. Ann. Deaf 147: 18–29.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, N. E., and Coltheart, M. (2001). Routes to Reading Success and Failure: Toward an Integrated Cognitive Psychology of Atypical Reading, Psychology Press, Philadelphia, PA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kampfe, C. M., and Turecheck, A. G. (1987). Reading achievement of prelingually deaf students and its relationship to parental method of communication. Am. Ann. Deaf 132: 11–15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, L. P. (2003a). The importance of processing automaticity and temporary storage capacity to the differences in comprehension between skilled and less skilled college-age deaf readers. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 8: 230–249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, L. P. (2003b). Considerations for designing practice for deaf readers. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 8: 171–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • King, C., and Quigley, S. (1985). Reading and Deafness. College-Hill, San Diego, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krakow, R. A., and Hanson, V. L. (1985). Deaf signers and serial recall in the visual modality: Memory for signs, finger spelling, and print. Memory Cognition 13: 265–272.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LaSasso, C., and Davey, B. (1987). The relationship between lexical knowledge and reading comprehension for prelingually profound hearing impaired students. Volta Rev. 89: 211–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A. M. (1992). The relation of speech to reading and writing. In Frost, R., and Katz, L. (eds.), Orthography, Phonology, Morphology, and Meaning. Advances in Psychology, North-Holland, Oxford, pp. 167–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lillo-Martin, D., Hanson, V., and Smith, S. (1992). Deaf readers’ comprehension of relative clauses. Appl. Psycholing. 13: 13–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marschark, M., and Everhart, V. S. (1997). Relations of language and cognition: What do deaf children tell us? In Marschark, M., Siple, P., Lillo-Martin, D., Campbell, R., et al. (eds.), Relation of Language and Thought: The View From Sign Language and Deaf Children, Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (1997). The effect of communication mode on the development of phonemic awareness in prelingually deaf students. J. Speech Hearing Res. 40: 1151–1163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2000). Syntactic and semantic processing in deaf and hearing readers. Am. Ann. Deaf 145: 436–448.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2001). Communication mode and the Information processing capacity of Hebrew readers with prelingually acquired deafness. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 13: 83–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2002a). Communication mode and the processing of printed words: Evidence from readers with prelingually acquired deafness. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 7: 312–329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2002b). Another look at the STM capacity of prelingually-deafened individuals and its relation to reading comprehension. Am. Ann. Deaf 147: 56–70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2004a). Processing of written words by individuals with prelingual deafness. J. Speech, Language, Hearing Res. 47: 979–989.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2004b). Processing of written word and non-word visual information by individuals with prelingual deafness. J. Speech, Language, Hearing Res. 47: 990–1000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2004c). The word decoding strategies of Hebrew readers with and without hearing impairments: Some insight from an associative learning task. Reading Writing: Interdisc. J. 17: 823–845.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2005a). Reading comprehension and its relation to the quality of functional hearing: Evidence from readers with different functional hearing abilities. Am. Ann. Deaf 150, 305–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2005b). What the word recognition skills of prelingually deafened readers tell about the roots of dyslexia. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 17, 369–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2006). What the processing of real words and pseudo-homophones tell about the development of orthographic knowledge in prelingually deafened individuals. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 11, 21–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Musselman, C. (2000). How do children who can’t hear learn to read an alphabetic script? A review of the literature on reading and deafness. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 5: 11–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, D. C., and Leutke-Stahlman, B. (2002). Phonological awareness: One key to the reading proficiency of deaf children. Am. Ann. Deaf 147: 11–19.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, C. A., and Caramazza, A. (2004). Orthographic structure and deaf spelling errors: Syllables, letter frequency, and speech. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 57: 385–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Padden, C. A. (1993). Lessons to be learned from the young deaf orthographer. Ling. Educ. 5: 71–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Padden, C. A., and Hanson, V. (2000). Search for the missing link: The development of skilled reading in deaf children. In Emmorey, K., and Lane, H. (eds.), The Signs of Language Revised: An Anthology to Honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 435–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parasnis, I., and Whitaker, H. A. (1992, April). Do deaf signers access phonological information in English words: Evidence from rhyme judgments, Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association Annual Meeting, San Francisco, CA.

  • Paul, P. (2001). Language and Deafness, 3rd edn., Singular Publishing, San Diego, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., and Sandak, R. (2000). Reading optimally builds on spoken language: Implications for deaf readers. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 5: 32–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Report of the National Reading Panel (2000). Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence-Based Assessment of the Scientific Research Literature on Reading and its Implications for Reading Instruction, NIH Publication No. 00–4769, U.S. Government Printing Office, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shand, M. A. (1982). Sign based short-term coding of American Sign Language signs and printed English words by congenitally deaf signers. Cogn. Psychol. 14: 1–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 55: 151–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siedlecki, T., Votaw, M. C., Bonvillian, J. D., and Jordan, K. I. (1990). The effect of manual interference and reading level of deaf subjects’ recall of word lists. Appl. Psycholing. 11: 185–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., and Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children, National Academic Press, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strassman, B., Kretschmer, R., and Bilsky, L. (1987). The instantiation of general terms by deaf adolescents/adults. J. Commun. Disord. 20: 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sutcliffe, A., Dowker, A., and Campbell, R. (1999). Deaf children’s spelling: Does it show sensitivity to phonology? J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 4: 111–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Transler, C., Leybaert, J., and Gomert, J. E. (1999). Do deaf children use phonological syllables as reading units? J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 4: 124–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., and Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1983). Silent reading: Insight from second-generation deaf readers. Cogn. Psychol. 15: 39–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walters, G. S., and Doehring, D. B. (1990). Reading acquisition in congenitally deaf children who communicate orally: Insights from an analysis of component reading, language, and memory skills. In Carr, T. H., and Levy, B. A. (eds.), Reading and its Development: Component Skills Approaches Academic Press, San Diego, CA, (pp. 323–373).

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisel, A. (1984). Perception of nonverbal messages with emotional content by hearing and deaf young adults. Dissert. Abstr. Int. 44(12-A): 3661–3662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwiebel, A. (1987). More on the effects of early manual communication on the cognitive development of deaf children. Am. Ann. Deaf 132: 16–20.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Miller.

Appendices

 

Appendix C

Table C.I. Examples of Sentences Used to Assess the Participants’ Reading Comprehension

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Miller, P. What the Visual Word Recognition Skills of Prelingually Deafened Readers Tell About Their Reading Comprehension Problems. J Dev Phys Disabil 18, 91–121 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-006-9002-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-006-9002-z

KEY WORDS:

Navigation