Alexander, P. A. (2019). Individual differences in college-age learners: The importance of relational reasoning for learning and assessment in higher education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 416–428. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12264.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Amd, M., & Roche, B. (2018). Assessing the effects of a relational training intervention on fluid intelligence among a sample of socially disadvantaged children in Bangladesh. The Psychological Record, 68(2), 141–149. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0273-4.
Article
Google Scholar
Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2015). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(2), 366–377. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0699-x.
Article
Google Scholar
Bergold, S., & Steinmayr, R. (2018). Personality and intelligence interact in the prediction of academic achievement. Journal of Intelligence, 6(2), 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6020027.
Article
Google Scholar
Blieszner, R., Willis, S. L., & Baltes, P. B. (1981). Training research in aging on the fluid ability of inductive reasoning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2(3), 247–265. https://doi.org/10.1016/0193-3973(81)90005-8.
Article
Google Scholar
Buschkuehl, M., Hernandez-Garcia, L., Jaeggi, S. M., Bernard, J. A., & Jonides, J. (2014). Neural effects of short-term training on working memory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(1), 147–160. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0244-9.
Article
Google Scholar
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., Colbert, D., Stewart, I., & Grey, I. M. (2016). A relational frame skills training intervention to increase general intelligence and scholastic aptitude. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 222–235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.03.0011041-6080.
Article
Google Scholar
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). A relational frame training intervention to raise intelligence quotients: A pilot study. The Psychological Record, 61(2), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395755.
Article
Google Scholar
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & O’Hora, D. (2010). Relational frame theory and human intelligence. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11(1), 37–51. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2010.11434333.
Article
Google Scholar
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Ahmetoglu, G., & Furnham, A. (2008). Little more than personality: Dispositional determinants of test anxiety (the Big Five, core self-evaluations, and self-assessed intelligence). Learning and Individual Differences, 18(2), 258–263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2007.09.002.
Article
Google Scholar
Chooi, W.-T., & Thompson, L. A. (2012). Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults. Intelligence, 40(6), 531–542. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.07.004.
Article
Google Scholar
Clark, C. M., Lawlor-Savage, L., & Goghari, V. M. (2017). Working memory training in healthy young adults: Support for the null from a randomized comparison to active and passive control groups. PLoS ONE, 12(5), e0177707.
Article
Google Scholar
Colbert, D., Dobutowitsch, M., Roche, B., & Brophy, C. (2017). The proxy-measurement of intelligence quotients using a relational skills abilities index. Learning and Individual Differences, 57, 114–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.010.
Article
Google Scholar
Colom, R., & Román, F. J. (2018). Enhancing Intelligence: From the Group to the Individual. Journal of Intelligence, 6(1), 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6010011.
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Colom, R., Román, F. J., Abad, F. J., Shih, P. C., Privado, J., Froufe, M., et al. (2013). Adaptive n-back training does not improve fluid intelligence at the construct level: Gains on individual tests suggest that training may enhance visuospatial processing. Intelligence, 41(5), 712–727. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.09.002.
Article
Google Scholar
Conway, A. R. A., & Kovacs, K. (2015). New and emerging models of human intelligence. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6(5), 419–426. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1356.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Davies, G., Lam, M., Harris, S. E., Trampush, J. W., Luciano, M., …, Hill, W. D., et al. (2018). Study of 300,486 individuals identifies 148 independent genetic loci influencing general cognitive function. Nature Communications, 9(1), 2098. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-04362-x.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Davis, T., Goldwater, M., & Giron, J. (2017). From concrete examples to abstract relations: The rostrolateral prefrontal cortex integrates novel examples into relational categories. Cerebral Cortex, 27(4), 2652–2670. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw099.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 880–896. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (2013). Advances in relational frame theory: Research and application. Oakland, CA: Context Press.
Google Scholar
Everaert, M. B. H., Huybregts, M. A. C., Chomsky, N., Berwick, R. C., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2015). Structures, not strings: Linguistics as part of the cognitive sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(12), 729–743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.09.008.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Goldwater, M. B., & Schalk, L. (2016). Relational categories as a bridge between cognitive and educational research. Psychological Bulletin, 142(7), 729–757. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000043.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gottfredson, L. S., & Deary, I. J. (2004). Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(1), 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301001.x.
Article
Google Scholar
Haier, R. J. (2016). The neuroscience of intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Halford, G. S., Bain, J. D., Maybery, M. T., & Andrews, G. (1998). Induction of relational schemas: Common processes in reasoning and complex learning. Cognitive Psychology, 35(3), 201–245. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0679.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hayes, J., & Stewart, I. (2016). Comparing the effects of derived relational training and computer coding on intellectual potential in school-age children. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(3), 397–411. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12114.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Hill, W. D., Marioni, R. E., Maghzian, O., Ritchie, S. J., Hagenaars, S. P., …, McIntosh, A. M. (2018a). A combined analysis of genetically correlated traits identifies 187 loci and a role for neurogenesis and myelination in intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry, 24, 169–181. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-017-0001-5.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Hill, W. D., Arslan, R. C., Xia, C., Luciano, M., Amador, C.,Navarro, P., …,Penke, L. (2018b). Genomic analysis of family data reveals additional genetic effects on intelligence and personality. Molecular Psychiatry, 23, 2347–2362. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-017-0005-1.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Ivcevic, Z., & Brackett, M. (2014). Predicting school success: Comparing conscientiousness, grit, and emotion regulation ability. Journal of Research in Personality, 52(10), 29–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.06.005.
Article
Google Scholar
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Shah, P. (2011). Short-and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(25), 10081–10086. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1103228108.
Article
Google Scholar
Jaeggi, S. M., Studer-Luethi, B., Buschkuehl, M., Su, Y.-F., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2010). The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning—Implications for training and transfer. Intelligence, 38(6), 625–635. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2010.09.001.
Article
Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2007). The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(2), 135–187. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07001185.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kaptchuk, T. J. (2001). The double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial: Gold standard or golden calf? Journal of Clinical Epidemiology (Vol. 54). Retrieved from https://www.medschool.lsuhsc.edu/internal_medicine/residency/docs/JC 2015-03 Gold standard or golden calf_Nesh.PDF
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman brief intelligence test. Hoboken: Wiley Online Library.
Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., Quilty, L. C., Grazioplene, R. G., Hirsh, J. B., Gray, J. R., Peterson, J. B., et al. (2016). Openness to experience and intellect differentially predict creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Journal of Personality, 84(2), 248–258. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12156.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kishita, N., Ohtsuki, T., & Stewart, I. (2013). The Training and Assessment of Relational Precursors and Abilities (TARPA): A follow-up study with typically developing children. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 2(1–2), 15–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2013.01.001.
Article
Google Scholar
McKey, R. H. (1985). The impact of head start on children, families and communities. Final Report of the Head Start Evaluation, Synthesis and Utilization Project. Retrieved from https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED263984.pdf
McLeod, D. R., Griffiths, R. R., Bigelow, G. E., & Yingling, J. (1982). An automated version of the digit symbol substitution test (DSST). Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 14(5), 463–466. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203313.
Article
Google Scholar
McLoughlin, S., Tyndall, I., & Pereira, A. (2018). Piloting a brief relational operant training program: Analyses of response latencies and intelligence test performance. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 19(2), 228–246. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2018.1507087.
Article
Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028228.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2016). There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is effective: A reply to Au et al. (2014) and Karbach and Verhaeghen (2014). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(1), 324–330. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0862-z.
Article
Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., Redick, T. S., & Hulme, C. (2016). Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer” evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 512–534. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616635612.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Mohammed, S., Flores, L., Deveau, J., Hoffing, R. C., Phung, C., Parlett, C. M., …, Buschkuehl, M. (2017). The benefits and challenges of implementing motivational features to boost cognitive training outcome. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 1(4), 491–507. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0047-y.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Moran, L., Walsh, L., Stewart, I., McElwee, J., & Ming, S. (2015). Correlating derived relational responding with linguistic and cognitive ability in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 19, 32–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.12.015.
Article
Google Scholar
Mulhern, T., Stewart, I., & Elwee, J. M. (2017). Investigating relational framing of categorization in young children. Psychological Record, 67(4), 519–536. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0255-y.
Article
Google Scholar
Mulhern, T., Stewart, I., & McElwee, J. (2018). Facilitating relational framing of classification in young children. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 8, 55–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.04.001.
Article
Google Scholar
O’Hora, D., Pelaez, M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2005). Derived relational responding and performance in verbal subtests of the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 55, 155–175. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395504.
Article
Google Scholar
O’Hora, D., Peláez, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Rae, G., Robinson, K., & Chaudhary, T. (2008). Temporal relations and intelligence : Correlating relational performance with performance on the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 58(1), 569–584. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395638.
Article
Google Scholar
O’Toole, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Murphy, C., O’Connor, J., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). Relational flexibility and human intelligence: Extending the remit of skinner’s verbal behavior. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 9(1), 1–17.
Google Scholar
Park, J., & Brannon, E. M. (2016). How to interpret cognitive training studies: A reply to Lindskog & Winman. Cognition, 150, 247–251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.02.012.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Parra, I., & Ruiz, F. J. (2016). The effect on intelligence quotient of training fluency in relational frames of coordination. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 16(1), 1–12.
Google Scholar
Peijnenborgh, J. C. A. W., Hurks, P. M., Aldenkamp, A. P., Vles, J. S. H., & Hendriksen, J. G. M. (2016). Efficacy of working memory training in children and adolescents with learning disabilities: A review study and meta-analysis. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 26(5–6), 645–672. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2015.1026356.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & Von Stumm, S. (2018). The new genetics of intelligence. Nature Reviews Genetics, 19(3), 148–159. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg.2017.104.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Rabipour, S., & Raz, A. (2012). Training the brain: Fact and fad in cognitive and behavioral remediation. Brain and Cognition, 79(2), 159–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2012.02.006.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ramani, G. B., Jaeggi, S. M., Daubert, E. N., & Buschkuehl, M. (2017). Domain-specific and domain-general training to improve kindergarten children’s mathematics. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 3(2), 468–495. https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.v3i2.31.
Article
Google Scholar
Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., ..., Engle, R. W. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 359-379. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029082.
Article
Google Scholar
Ritchie, S. (2015). Intelligence: All that matters. London: Hodder & Stoughton.
Google Scholar
Ruiz, F. J., & Luciano, C. (2011). Cross-domain analogies as relating derived relations among two separate relational networks. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95(3), 369–385. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.95-369.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Sala, G., Foley, J. P., & Gobet, F. (2017). The effects of chess instruction on pupils’ cognitive and academic skills: State of the art and theoretical challenges. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00238.
Article
Google Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017a). When the music’s over. Does music skill transfer to children’s and young adolescents’ cognitive and academic skills? A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 20, 55–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2016.11.005.
Article
Google Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017b). Working memory training in typically developing children: A meta-analysis of the available evidence. Developmental Psychology, 53(4), 671–685. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000265.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Salminen, T., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2012). On the impacts of working memory training on executive functioning. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 166. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00166.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Sandoz, E. K., & Hebert, E. R. (2017). Using derived relational responding to model statistics learning across participants with varying degrees of statistics anxiety. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 18(1), 113–131. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2016.1146552.
Article
Google Scholar
Savage, J. E., Jansen, P. R., Stringer, S., Watanabe, K., Bryois, J., de Leeuw, C. A., …, Posthuma, D. (2018). Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nature Genetics, 50, 912–919. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0152-6.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Schulte, M. J., Ree, M. J., & Carretta, T. R. (2004). Emotional intelligence: Not much more than g and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(5), 1059–1068. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.11.014.
Article
Google Scholar
Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F., & Bühner, M. (2015). Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educational Psychologist, 50(2), 138–166. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2015.1036274.
Article
Google Scholar
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 628–654. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027473.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14(1), 5–13. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.1401.05.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2016). Do “Brain-Training” programs work? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17(3), 103–186. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100616661983.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Book
Google Scholar
Smith-Woolley, E., Pingault, J.-B., Selzam, S., Rimfeld, K., Krapohl, E., von Stumm, S., …, Plomin, R. (2018). Differences in exam performance between pupils attending selective and non-selective schools mirror the genetic differences between them. Npj Science of Learning, 3(1), 3. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-018-0019-8.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Soveri, A., Karlsson, E., Waris, O., Grönholm-Nyman, P., & Laine, M. (2017). Pattern of near transfer effects following working memory training with a dual n-back task. Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 240–252. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000370.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Stepankova, H., Lukavsky, J., Buschkuehl, M., Kopecek, M., Ripova, D., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2014). The malleability of working memory and visuospatial skills: A randomized controlled study in older adults. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1049-1059. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034913.
Article
Google Scholar
Stephenson, C. L., & Halpern, D. F. (2013). Improved matrix reasoning is limited to training on tasks with a visuospatial component. Intelligence, 41(5), 341–357. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.05.006.
Article
Google Scholar
Strenze, T. (2007). Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence, 35(5), 401–426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.004.
Article
Google Scholar
Strobach, T., & Huestegge, L. (2017). Evaluating the effectiveness of commercial brain game training with working-memory tasks. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 1(4), 539–558. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0053-0.
Article
Google Scholar
Thirus, J., Starbrink, M., & Jansson, B. (2016). Relational frame theory, mathematical and logical skills: A multiple exemplar training intervention to enhance intellectual performance. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 16(2), 141–155.
Google Scholar
Thompson, T. W., Waskom, M. L., Garel, K.-L. A., Cardenas-Iniguez, C., Reynolds, G. O., Winter, R., …, Alvarez, G. A. (2013). Failure of working memory training to enhance cognition or intelligence. PLoS ONE, 8(5), e63614. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0063614.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Titz, C., & Karbach, J. (2014). Working memory and executive functions: effects of training on academic achievement. Psychological Research, 78, 852–868. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0537-1.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Van Der Maas, H. L. J., Dolan, C. V., Grasman, R. P. P. P., Wicherts, J. M., Huizenga, H. M., & Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2006). A dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review, 113(4), 842–861. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.4.842
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Vizcaíno-Torres, R. M., Ruiz, F. J., Luciano, C., López-López, J. C., Barbero-Rubio, A., & Gil, E. (2015). The effect of relational training on intelligence quotient: A case study. Psicothema, 27, 120–127. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2014.149.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., & Hayes, S. C. (2018). Evolution and contextual behavioral science: An integrated framework for understanding, predicting, and influencing human behavior. New Harbinger: Context Press.
Google Scholar
Zabaneh, D., Krapohl, E., Gaspar, H. A., Curtis, C., Lee, S. H., Patel, H., …, Breen, G. (2017). A genome-wide association study for extremely high intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry 23, 1226–1232. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.121.
Article
Google Scholar