Assumptions of the Deficit Model Type of Thinking: Ignorance, Attitudes, and Science Communication in the Debate on Genetic Engineering in Agriculture

Articles

Abstract

This paper spells out and discusses four assumptions of the deficit model type of thinking. The assumptions are: First, the public is ignorant of science. Second, the public has negative attitudes towards (specific instances of) science and technology. Third, ignorance is at the root of these negative attitudes. Fourth, the public’s knowledge deficit can be remedied by one-way science communication from scientists to citizens. It is argued that there is nothing wrong with ignorance-based explanations per se. Ignorance accounts at least partially for many cases of opposition to specific instances of science and technology. Furthermore, more attention needs to be paid to the issue of relevance. In regard to the evaluation of a scientific experiment, a technology, or a product, the question is not only “who knows best?,” but also “what knowledge is relevant and to what extent?.” Examples are drawn primarily from the debate on genetic engineering in agriculture.

Keywords

Deficit model Ignorance Attitudes Science communication Genetic engineering Nanotechnology 

References

  1. Ahteensuu, M., & Siipi, H. (2009). A critical assessment of public consultations on GMOs in the European Union. Environmental Values, 18(2), 129–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bonny, S. (2003). Why are most Europeans opposed to GMOs? Factors explaining rejection in France and Europe. Electronic Journal of Biotechnology, 6, 50–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Brown, S. (2009). The new deficit model. Nature Nanotechnology, 4, 609–611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bubela, T., et al. (2009). Science communication reconsidered. Nature Biotechnology, 27, 514–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bucchi, M., & Neresini, F. (2002). Biotech remains unloved by the more informed. Nature, 416, 261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cook, G., Pieri, E., & Robbins, P. T. (2004). The scientists think and the public feels: Expert perceptions of the discourse of GM food. Discourse and Society, 15(4), 433–449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Currall, S. C. (2008). New insights into public perceptions. Nature Nanotechnology, 4, 79–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Currall, S. C., et al. (2006). What drives public acceptance of nanotechnology? Nature Nanotechnology, 1, 153–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Currall, S. C., et al. (2007). Authors’ response. Nature Nanotechnology, 2, 327–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dickson, D. (2005). The case of ‘deficit model’ of science communication. Science and Development Network. http://www.scidev.net/en/editorials/the-case-for-a-deficit-model-of-science-communic.html. Accessed on November 17, 2010.
  11. Durant, J. R., Evans, G. A., & Thomas, G. P. (1989). The public understanding of science. Nature, 340, 11–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Editors. (2009). A seedy practice. Scientific American, 301(2), 22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. EEA = European Environment Agency. (2001). Late lessons from early warnings: The precautionary principle 18962000. http://reports.eea.eu.int/environmental_issue_report_2001_22/en/Issue_Report_No_22.pdf. Accessed on November 17, 2010.
  14. Einsiedel, E. (2007). Editorial: Of publics and science. Public Understanding of Science, 16(1), 5–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. European Commission. (2005). Special eurobarometer 224: Europeans, science and technology.Google Scholar
  16. European Commission. (2008a). Special eurobarometer 295: Attitudes of European citizens towards the environment. Google Scholar
  17. European Commission. (2008b). Qualitative study on the image of science and the research policy of the European Union.Google Scholar
  18. European Commission. (2010). Special eurobarometer 340: Science and technology.Google Scholar
  19. Evans, R. (2008). The sociology of expertise: The distribution of social fluency. Sociology Compass, 2(1), 281–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Evans, G. A., & Durant, J. R. (1995). The relationship between knowledge and attitudes in the public understanding of science in Britain. Public Understanding of Science, 4, 57–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Foot, P. (1958). Moral arguments. Mind, 67(268), 502–513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Frankena, W. K. (1939). The naturalistic fallacy. Mind, 48(192), 464–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gaskell, G., et al. (2006). Europeans and biotechnology in 2005: Patterns and trends: Eurobarometer 64.3. http://www.ec.europa.eu/research/press/2006/pdf/pr1906_eb_64_3_final_report-may2006_en.pdf. Accessed on November 17, 2010.
  24. Gaskell, G., Allum, N., & Stares, S. (2003). Europeans and biotechnology in 2002: Eurobarometer 58.0. Google Scholar
  25. Gaskell, G., et al. (1999). Worlds apart? The reception of genetically modified foods in Europe and the US. Nature, 285, 384–387.Google Scholar
  26. Gregory, J., & Lock, J. (2008). The evolution of ‘Public Understanding of Science’: Public engagement as a tool of science policy in the UK. Sociology Compass, 2(4), 1252–1265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hails, R., & Kinderlerer, J. (2003). The GM public debate: Context and communication strategies. Nature Reviews, 4, 819–825.Google Scholar
  28. Hansen, J., et al. (2003). Beyond the knowledge deficit: Recent research into lay and expert attitudes to food risks. Appetite, 41, 111–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hansson, S. O. (2008). Science and pseudo-science. Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/pseudo-science/. Accessed on September 27, 2010.
  30. INRA (Europe)—ECOSA. (2000). Eurobarometer 52.1.: The Europeans and biotechnology.Google Scholar
  31. Irwin, A., & Wynne, B. (Eds.). (1996). Misunderstood science? The public reconstruction of science and technology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Jones, M. (2008). Fearing the fear of nanotechnology. Nature (Dec. 9), 1290.Google Scholar
  33. Kahan, D. M., et al. (2009). Cultural cognition of the risks and benefits of nanotechnology. Nature Nanotechnology, 4, 87–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Kvakkestad, V., et al. (2007). Scientists’ perspectives on the deliberate release of GM crops. Environmental Values, 16, 79–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kvanvig, J. (2003). The value of knowledge and the pursuit of understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lidskog, R. (2008). Scientised citizens and democratised science: Re-assessing the expert-lay divide. Journal of Risk Research, 11(1–2), 69–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Louët, S. (2001). EC study reveals an informed public. Nature, 19, 15–16.Google Scholar
  39. Marris, C., et al. (2001). Public perceptions of agricultural biotechnologies in Europe. Final report of the PABE research project. Commissioned by the EC.Google Scholar
  40. Martin, S., & Tait, J. (1992). Attitudes of selected public groups in the UK to biotechnology. In J. Durant (Ed.), Biotechnology in public: A review of recent research (pp. 28–41). London: Science Museum.Google Scholar
  41. Midden, C., et al. (2002). The structure of public perceptions. In M. W. Bauer & G. Gaskell (Eds.), Biotechnology: The making of a global controversy (pp. 203–223). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  42. National Science Board. (2004). Science and engineering indicators 2004. Two volumes. Arlington, VA: National Science Foundation (volume 1, NSB 04-1; volume 2, NSB 04-1A).Google Scholar
  43. National Science Board. (2010). Science and engineering indicators 2010. Arlington, VA: National Science Foundation (NSB 10–01).Google Scholar
  44. O’Neill, O. (2002). Autonomy and trust in bioethics. Port Chester, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Pardo, R., & Calvo, F. (2006). Are Europeans really antagonistic to biotech? Nature, 24(4), 393–395.Google Scholar
  46. Pardo, R., Midden, C., & Miller, J. D. (2002). Attitudes toward biotechnology in the European Union. Journal of Biotechnology, 98, 9–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Peters, H. P. (2000). From information to attitudes? Thoughts on the relationship between knowledge about science and technology and attitudes toward technology. In M. Dierkes & C. von Grote (Eds.), Between understanding and trust: The public, science and technology (pp. 265–286). Amsterdam: Harwood.Google Scholar
  48. Royal Society. (1985). The public understanding of science: Report of the royal society’s ad hoc group. London: The Royal Society.Google Scholar
  49. Royal Society. (2004). Science in society report. London: The Royal Society.Google Scholar
  50. Savadori, L., et al. (2004). Expert and public perceptions of risk from biotechnology. Risk Analysis, 24(5), 1289–1299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Scheufele, D. A., et al. (2008a). Religious beliefs and public attitudes toward nanotechnology in Europe and the United States. Nature Nanotechnology, 4, 91–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Scheufele, D. A., et al. (2008b). Scientists worry about some risks more than the public. Nature Nanotechnology, 2, 732–734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Searle, J. R. (1964). How to derive “ought” from “is”. Philosophical Review, 73(1), 43–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Slovic, P., Peters, E., Finucane, M. L., & MacGregor, D. (2005). Affect, risk, and decision making. Health Psychology, 24(4), S35–S40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Sturgis, P., & Allum, N. (2004). Science in society: Re-evaluating the deficit model of public attitudes. Public Understanding of Science, 13, 55–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Sturgis, P., Cooper, H., & Fife-Schaw, C. (2005). Attitudes to biotechnology: Estimating the opinions of a better-informed public. New Genetics and Society, 24(1), 31–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Walton, D. (1992). Nonfallacious arguments from ignorance. American Philosophical Quarterly, 29(4), 381–387.Google Scholar
  58. Waltz, E. (2009). GM crops: Battlefield. Nature, 461, 27–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Whiteside, K. H. (2006). Precautionary politics: Principle and practice in confronting environmental risk. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  60. Wiener, J. B., & Rogers, M. D. (2002). Comparing precaution in the United States and Europe. Journal of Risk Research, 5, 317–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Wintle, B., Burgman, M., & Fidler, F. (2007). How fast should nanotechnology advance? Nature Nanotechnology, 2, 327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Wright, N., & Nerlich, B. (2006). Use of the deficit model in a shared culture of argumentation: The case of foot and mouth science. Public Understanding of Science, 15, 331–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Wynne, B. (1991). Knowledges in context. Science, Technology, and Human Values, 16, 111–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Wynne, B. (1996). May the sheep safely graze? A reflexive view of the expert-lay knowledge divide. In S. Lash, B. Szerszynski, & B. Wynne (Eds.), Risk, environment and modernity: Towards a new ecology (pp. 44–83). London: Sage.Google Scholar
  65. Wynne, B. (2006). Public engagement as means of restoring public trust in science–Hitting the notes, but missing the music. Community Genetics, 9, 211–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Ziman, J. (1991). Public understanding of science. Science, Technology, and Human Values, 16, 99–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of Turku, Public Choice Research Centre (PCRC)TurkuFinland

Personalised recommendations