Abu-Akel, A., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2011). Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind. Neuropsychologia, 49(11), 2971–2984. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.012
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Adams, R., Ambady, N., Macrae, C., & Kleck, R. (2006). Emotional expressions forecast approach-avoidance behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 30(2), 177–186. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9020-2
Article
Google Scholar
Atherton, G., & Cross, L. (2018). seeing more than human: Autism and anthropomorphic theory of mind. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00528
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Bai, Q., Dan, Q., Mu, Z., & Yang, M. (2019). A systematic review of emoji: Current research and future perspectives. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02221
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Lombardo, M., Auyeung, B., Ashwin, E., Chakrabarti, B., & Knickmeyer, R. (2011). Why are autism spectrum conditions more prevalent in males? Plos Biology, 9(6), 100–108. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001081
Article
Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The systemizing quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 361–374. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1206
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Bullmore, E., Wheelwright, S., Ashwin, C., & Williams, S. (2000). The amygdala theory of autism. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(3), 355–364. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00011-7
Article
Google Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
Article
Google Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Article
Google Scholar
Bertrams, A. (2021). Internal reliability, homogeneity, and factor structure of the ten-item Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ-10) with two additional response categories. Experimental Results, 2, E3. https://doi.org/10.1017/exp.2020.70
Article
Google Scholar
Bird, G., & Cook, R. (2013). Mixed emotions: The contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Translational Psychiatry, 23(7), e285. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2013.61
Article
Google Scholar
Boutet, I., LeBlanc, M., Chamberland, J. A., & Collin, C. A. (2021). Emojis influence emotional communication, social attributions, and information processing. Computers in Human Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106722
Article
Google Scholar
Brewer, R., Happé, F., Cook, R., & Bird, G. (2015). Commentary on ‘Autism, oxytocin and interoception’: Alexithymia, not autism spectrum disorders, is the consequence of interoceptive failure. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 56, 348–353.
Article
Google Scholar
British Psychological Society. (2014). Code of Human Research Ethics. BPS.
Google Scholar
Burke, M., Kraut, R., & Williams, D. (2010). Social use of computer-mediated communication by adults on the autism spectrum. Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work, CSCW. https://doi.org/10.1145/1718918.1718991
Article
Google Scholar
Bürkner, P.-C., & Vuorre, M. (2019). Ordinal regression models in psychology: A tutorial. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2(1), 77–101. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918823199
Article
Google Scholar
Carton, J., Kessler, E., & Pape, C. (1999). Nonverbal decoding skills and relationship well-being in adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23(1), 91–100. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021339410262
Article
Google Scholar
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E., & Schultz, R. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 231–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016). Social attention in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 48, 79–93.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Christensen, R. H. B. (2019). Ordinal—Regression models for ordinal data. R package version 2019.4–25. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/ordinal/index.html
Cook, R., Brewer, R., Shah, P., & Bird, G. (2013). Alexithymia, not autism, predicts poor recognition of emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 24(5), 723–732. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612463582
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cooke, J. E., Stuart-Parrigon, S. E., Movahed-Abtahi, M., Koehn, A. J., & Kerns, K. A. (2016). Children’s emotion understanding and mother-child attachment: A meta-analysis. Emotion, 16(8), 1102–1106. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000221
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cooper, R. M., Rowe, A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Ludwig, C. (2009). No reliable effects of emotional facial expression, adult attachment orientation, or anxiety on the allocation of visual attention in the spatial cueing paradigm. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(4), 643–652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.03.005
Article
Google Scholar
Crompton, C. J., Fletcher-Watson, S., & Ropar, D. (2019, September 24). Autistic peer to peer information transfer is highly effective. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/j4knx
Derks, D., Bos, A., & von Grumbkow, J. (2007). Emoticons and online message interpretation. Social Science Computer Review, 26(3), 379–388. https://doi.org/10.1177/0894439307311611
Article
Google Scholar
Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1986). Why faces are and are not special: An effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(2), 107–117. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.115.2.107
Article
Google Scholar
Edey, R., Cook, J., Brewer, R., Johnson, M. H., Bird, G., & Press, C. (2016). Interaction takes two: Typical adults exhibit mind-blindness towards those with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(7), 879–885.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Freisen, W., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face. Pergamon.
Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17(2), 124–129. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030377
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1976). Measuring facial movement. Environmental Psychology and Nonverbal Behavior, 1(1), 56–75. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf01115465
Article
Google Scholar
Farah, M., Wilson, K., Drain, M., & Tanaka, J. (1998). What is “special” about face perception? Psychological Review, 105(3), 482–498. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.105.3.482
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fiene, L., & Brownlow, C. (2015). Investigating interoception and body awareness in adults with and without autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 8(6), 709–716. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1486
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fletcher-Watson, S., & Bird, G. (2020). Autism and empathy: What are the real links? Autism, 24(1), 3–6. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319883506
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Folstein, S., & Rosen-Sheidley, B. (2001). Genetics of austim: Complex aetiology for a heterogeneous disorder. Nature Reviews Genetics, 2(12), 943–955. https://doi.org/10.1038/35103559
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Forstmeier, W., Wagenmakers, E., & Parker, T. (2016). Detecting and avoiding likely false-positive findings—A practical guide. Biological Reviews, 92(4), 1941–1968. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12315
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Frank, M., & Stennett, J. (2001). The forced-choice paradigm and the perception of facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 75–85. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.75
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fridlund, A. (1994). APA PsycNET Mobile menu human facial expression: An evolutionary view (pp. 140–186). Academic Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds—A biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692–1695. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.286.5445.1692
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 459–473. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1218
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Fullwood, C., Orchard, L., & Floyd, S. (2013). Emoticon convergence in Internet chat rooms. Social Semiotics, 23(5), 648–662. https://doi.org/10.1080/10350330.2012.739000
Article
Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2004). Understanding Interpersonal problems in autism: Interaction theory as an alternative to theory of mind. Philosophy, Psychiatry & Psychology, 11(3), 199–217. https://doi.org/10.1353/ppp.2004.0063
Article
Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A., Stevenson, J. L., & Dern, S. (2017). Specificity, contexts, and reference groups matter when assessing autistic traits. PLoS ONE, 12(2), e01711931.
Article
Google Scholar
Golarai, G., Grill-Spector, K., & Reiss, A. (2006). Autism and the development of face processing. Clinical Neuroscience Research, 6(3–4), 145–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cnr.2006.08.001
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
González-Ibáñez, R., Shah, C., & Córdova-Rubio, N. (2011). Smile! Studying expressivity of happiness as a synergic factor in collaborative information seeking. Proceedings of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 48(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1002/meet.2011.14504801171
Article
Google Scholar
Grynberg, D., Chang, B., Cornielle, O., Maurage, P., Vermeulen, N., Berthoz, S., & Luminet, O. (2012). Alexithymia and the processing of emotional facial expressions (EFEs): Systematic review, unanswered questions and further perspectives. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e42429. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042429
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review, 20, 290–322.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hill, E., Berthoz, S., & Frith, U. (2004). Brief Report: Cognitive Processing of Own Emotions in Individuals with Autistic Spectrum Disorder and in Their Relatives. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 229–235. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:jadd.0000022613.41399.14
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hobson, R. (1986). The autistic child’s appraisal of expressions of emotion: A further study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(5), 671–680. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1986.tb00191.x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H. (2014). Autism: Demise of the innate social orienting hypothesis. Current Biology, 24(1), R30–R31.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. (1996). A module for the visual representation of faces. NeuroImage, 3(3), S361. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(96)80363-6
Article
Google Scholar
Kaye, L., Malone, S., & Wall, H. (2017). Emojis: Insights, affordances, and possibilities for psychological science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(2), 66–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.007
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ketelaars, M. P., Velt, A. I., Mol, A., Swaab, H., & van Rijn, S. (2016). Emotion recognition and alexithymia in high functioning females with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 21, 51–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.09.006
Article
Google Scholar
Kidron, R., Kaganovskiy, L., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2018). Empathizing-systemizing cognitive styles: Effects of sex and academic degree. PLoS ONE, 13(3), e0194515. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0194515
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Kok, F., Groen, Y., Becke, M., Fuermaier, A., & Tucha, O. (2016). Self-reported empathy in adult women with autism spectrum disorders—A systematic mini review. PLoS ONE, 11(3), e0151568. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151568
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Lawson, J., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). Empathising and systemising in adults with and without Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 301–310. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:jadd.0000029552.42724.1b
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lawson, W. (2010). The passionate mind: How people with autism learn. Jessica Kingsley.
Google Scholar
Leman, P., Bremner, A., Parke, R., & Gauvin, M. (2012). Developmental psychology (pp. 232–275). McGraw-Hill Higher Education.
Google Scholar
Lenth, R., Singmann, H., Love, J., Buerkner, P., & Herve, M. (2020). Package ‘emmeans’. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/emmeans/emmeans.pdf
Li, H. (2013). Cultural Differences in Adult Attachment and Facial Emotion Recognition (Doctoral dissertation, University of Ulm). https://doi.org/10.18725/OPARU-2965
Liddell, T. M., & Kruschke, J. K. (2018). Analyzing ordinal data with metric models: What could possibly go wrong? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 79, 328–348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.08.009
Article
Google Scholar
Loomes, R., Hull, L., & Mandy, W. (2017). What is the male-to-female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(6), 466–474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013
Article
Google Scholar
Matuschek, H., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., Baayen, H., & Bates, D. (2017). Balancing type I error and power in linear mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 305–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.01.001
Article
Google Scholar
Mazurek, M. (2013). Social media use among adults with autism spectrum disorders. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1709–1714. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.004
Article
Google Scholar
McCullagh, P. (1980). Regression models for ordinal data. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 42(2), 109–127. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1980.tb01109
Article
Google Scholar
Milton, D. (2012). On the ontological status of autism: The double empathy problem. Disability and Society, 27(6), 883–887.
Article
Google Scholar
Mundy, P. (2018). A review of joint attention and social-cognitive brain systems in typical development and autism spectrum disorder. European Journal of Neuroscience, 47, 497–514.
Article
Google Scholar
Muresan, S., Gonzalez-Ibanez, R., Ghosh, D., & Wacholder, N. (2016). Identification of nonliteral language in social media: A case study on sarcasm. Journal of the Association for Information Science and Technology, 67(11), 2725–2737. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.23624
Article
Google Scholar
Murray, D., Lesser, M., & Lawson, W. (2005). Attention, monotropism and the diagnostic criteria for autism. Autism, 9, 139–156.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Nemiah, J. C., Freyberger, H., & Sifneos, P. E. (1976). Alexithymia: A view of the psychosomatic process. In O. W. Hill (Ed.), Modern trends in psychosomatic medicine (pp. 430–439). Butterworths.
Google Scholar
Oleszkiewicz, A., Karwowski, M., Pisanski, K., Sorokowski, P., Sobrado, B., & Sorokowska, A. (2017). Who uses emoticons? Data from 86 702 Facebook users. Personality and Individual Differences, 119, 289–295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.07.034
Article
Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1990). Are there emotion perception deficits in young autistic children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(3), 343–361. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb01574.x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pelphrey, K., & Carter, E. (2008). Charting the typical and atypical development of the social brain. Development and Psychopathology, 20(4), 1081–1102. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579408000515
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pelphrey, K., Sasson, N., Reznick, J., Paul, G., Goldman, B., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 249–261. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016374617369
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Chimpanzee theory of mind: Part I. Perception of causality and purpose in the child and chimpanzee. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 616–629. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00077050
Article
Google Scholar
Riordan, M., & Kreuz, R. (2010). Emotion encoding and interpretation in computer-mediated communication: Reasons for use. Computers in Human Behavior, 26(6), 1667–1673. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.06.015
Article
Google Scholar
Robus, C., Hand, C., Filik, R., & Pitchford, M. (2020). Investigating effects of emoji on neutral narrative text: Evidence from eye movements and perceived emotional valence. Computers in Human Behavior, 109, 106–361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106361
Article
Google Scholar
Rodrigues, D., Prada, M., Gaspar, R., Garrido, M., & Lopes, D. (2017). Lisbon Emoji and Emoticon Database (LEED): Norms for emoji and emoticons in seven evaluative dimensions. Behavior Research Methods, 50(1), 392–405. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0878-6
Article
Google Scholar
Rosset, D., Rondan, C., Da Fonseca, D., Santos, A., Assouline, B., & Deruelle, C. (2007). Typical emotion processing for cartoon but not for real faces in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(5), 919–925. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0465-2
Article
Google Scholar
Said, C., Haxby, J., & Todorov, A. (2011). Brain systems for assessing the affective value of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 366(1571), 1660–1670. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0351
Article
Google Scholar
Salminen, J. K., Saarijärvi, S., Äärelä, E., Toikka, T., & Kauhanen, J. (1999). Prevalence of alexithymia and its association with sociodemographic variables in the general population of Finland. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 46, 75–82.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sampietro, A. (2020). Use and interpretation of emoji in electronic-mediated communication: A survey. Visual Communication Quarterly, 27(1), 27–39. https://doi.org/10.1080/15551393.2019.1707086
Article
Google Scholar
Schultz, R. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 125–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.12.012
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sheppard, E., Pillai, D., Wong, G. T. L., Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2016). How easy is it to read the minds of people with autism spectrum disorder? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 1247–1254.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sivaratnam, C., Newman, L., Tonge, B., & Rinehart, N. (2015). Attachment and emotion processing in children with autism spectrum disorders: Neurobiological, neuroendocrine, and neurocognitive considerations. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2(2), 222–242. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-015-0048-7
Article
Google Scholar
Skovholt, K., Grønning, A., & Kankaanranta, A. (2014). The communicative functions of emoticons in workplace e-mails: :-). Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 19(4), 780–797. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12063
Article
Google Scholar
Swart, M., Kortekaas, R., & Aleman, A. (2009). Dealing with feelings: Characterization of trait alexithymia on emotion regulation strategies and cognitive-emotional processing. PLoS ONE, 4(6), e5751. Retrieved from http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0005751
Szabó, Z. G. (2019). Semantic Explanations. In E. Lepore & D. Sosa (Eds.), Oxford studies in philosophy of language (pp. 240–275). Oxford University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Taylor, J. E., Rousselet, G. A., Scheepers, C., & Sereno, S. C. (2021, August 3). Rating norms should be calculated from cumulative link mixed effects models. Retrieved Aug 23, 2021, from https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3vgwk
Taylor, E. C., Livingston, L. A., Clutterbuck, R. A., & Shah, P. (2020). Psychometric concerns with the 10-item Autism-Spectrum-Quotient (AQ10) as a measure of trait autism in the general population. Experimental Results, 1, E3. https://doi.org/10.1017/exp.2019.3
Article
Google Scholar
Thompson, D., & Filik, R. (2016). Sarcasm in written communication: Emoticons are efficient markers of intention. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 21(2), 105–120. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12156
Article
Google Scholar
Thye, M., Bednarz, H., Herringshaw, A., Sartin, E., & Kana, R. (2018). The impact of atypical sensory processing on social impairments in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 29, 151–167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.04.010
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Uljarevic, M., & Hamilton, A. (2012). Recognition of emotions in autism: A formal meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(7), 1517–1526. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1695-5
Article
Google Scholar
Valentine, T. (1988). Upside-down faces: A review of the effect of inversion upon face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 79(4), 471–491. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1988.tb02747.x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
van der Geest, J., Kemner, C., Camfferman, G., Verbaten, M., & van Engeland, H. (2001). Eye movements, visual attention, and autism: A saccadic reaction time study using the gap and overlap paradigm. Biological Psychiatry, 50(8), 614–619. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01070-8
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Walther, J. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction. Communication Research, 19(1), 52–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/009365092019001003
Article
Google Scholar
Walther, J., & D’Addario, K. (2001). The impacts of emoticons on message interpretation in computer-mediated communication. Social Science Computer Review, 19(3), 324–347. https://doi.org/10.1177/089443930101900307
Article
Google Scholar
Wang, Z. (2015). Theory of mind and children’s understanding of teaching and learning during early childhood. Cogent Education. https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186x.2015.1011973
Article
Google Scholar
Watkins, E., Zimmermann, Z., & Poling, A. (2014). The gender of participants in published research involving people with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8(2), 143–146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2013.10.010
Article
Google Scholar
Wellman, H. (1992). The Child’s theory of mind (pp. 100–105). The Mit Press.
Google Scholar
White, R. C., & Remington, A. (2019). Object personification in autism: This paper will be very sad if you don’t read it. Autism, 23(4), 1042–1045. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361318793408
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17(7), 592–598.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Willoughby, J., & Liu, S. (2018). Do pictures help tell the story? An experimental test of narrative and emojis in a health text message intervention. Computers in Human Behavior, 79, 75–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.031
Article
Google Scholar
Wolf, A. (2000). Emotional expression online: gender differences in emoticon use. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 3(5), 827–833. https://doi.org/10.1089/10949310050191809
Article
Google Scholar
World Health Organisation. (2004). ICD-10 (2nd ed., pp. 226–230).
Zaboski, B. A., & Storch, E. A. (2018). Comorbid autism spectrum disorder and anxiety disorders: A brief review. Future Neurology, 13(1), 31–37. https://doi.org/10.2217/fnl-2017-0030
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar