Skip to main content
Log in

Investigating the Use of World Knowledge During On-line Comprehension in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The on-line use of world knowledge during reading was examined in adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Both ASD and typically developed adults read sentences that included plausible, implausible and anomalous thematic relations, as their eye movements were monitored. No group differences in the speed of detection of the anomalous violations were found, but the ASD group showed a delay in detection of implausible thematic relations. These findings suggest that there are subtle differences in the speed of world knowledge processing during reading in ASD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th edn.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Åsberg, J., Kopp, S., Berg-Kelly, K., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Reading comprehension, word decoding and spelling in girls with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD): Performance and predictors. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 45(1), 61–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Åsberg, J., & Sandberg, A. D. (2012). Dyslexic, delayed, precocious or just normal? Word reading skills of children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Research in Reading, 35(1), 20–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Au-Yeung, S. K., Kaakinen, J. K., Liversedge, S. P., & Benson, V. (2015). Processing of written irony in autism spectrum disorder: An eye movement study. Autism Research, 8, 749–760. doi:10.1002/aur.1490.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benson, V., Castelhano, M. S., Au-Yeung, S. K., & Rayner, K. (2012). Eye movements reveal no immediate “WOW” (“which one’s weird”) effect in autism spectrum disorder. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(6), 1139–1150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benson, V., Castelhano, M. S., Howard, P. L., Latif, N., & Rayner, K. (2015). Looking, seeing and believing in autism: Eye movements reveal how subtle cognitive processing difference impact in the social domain. Autism Research. doi:10.1002/aur.1580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodner, K. E., Engelhardt, C. R., Minshew, N. J., & Williams, D. L. (2015). Making inferences: Comprehension of physical causality, intentionality, and emotions in discourse by high-functioning older children, adolescents, and adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(9), 2721–2733.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., & Bzishvili, S. (2013). Deconstructing Frith and Snowling’s homograph-reading task: Implications for autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(9), 1764–1773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., & Caruana, N. (2014). Reading for sound and reading for meaning in autism: Frith and Snowling (1983) revisited. In J. Arciuli & J. Brock (Eds.), Communication in autism (pp. 125–145). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., Norbury, C., Einav, S., & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-movements. Cognition, 108(3), 896–904.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, H. M., Oram-Cardy, J., & Johnson, A. (2013). A meta-analysis of the reading comprehension skills of individuals on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 932–955.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, G. N., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1988). Thematic roles and language comprehension. In W. Wilkins (Ed.), Thematic Relations (pp. 263–288). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caruana, N., & Brock, B. (2014). No association between autisitic traits and contextual influences on eye-movements during reading. PeerJ, 2, e446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741–748.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, M., Lazenby, A. L., & Lockyer, L. (2001). Inferential language in high-function children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 47–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drieghe, D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2008). Mislocated fixations can account for parafoveal-on-foveal effects in eye movements during reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(8), 1239–1249.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Engbert, R., Nuthmann, A., Richter, E. M., & Kliegl, R. (2005). SWIFT: a dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review, 112(4), 777.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, H. J., & Sanford, A. J. (2008). Anomalies in real and counterfactual worlds: An eye-movement investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 609–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (2012). The 38th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture. Why we need cognitive explanations of autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(11), 2073–2092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism—Beyond the theory of mind. Cognition, 50(1–3), 115–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Snowling, M. (1983). Reading for meaning and reading for sound in autistic and dyslexic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 329–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A. (1991). Cognitive-processes and mechanisms in language comprehension—The structure building framework. Psychology of Learning and Motivation-Advances in Research and Theory, 27, 217–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101(3), 371–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hahn, N., Snedeker, J., & Rabagliati, H. (2015). Rapid linguistic ambiguity resolution in young children with autism spectrum disorder: Eye tracking evidence for the limits of weak central coherence. Autism Research, 8, 717–726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hala, S., Pexman, P. M., & Glenwright, M. (2007). Priming the meaning of homographs in typically developing children and children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(2), 329–340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading homographs in context. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L. M., Clarke, P. J., & Snowling, M. J. (2011). Accessing and selecting word meaning in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(9), 964–973.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howard, P. L., Liversedge, S. P., & Benson, V. (2017). Benchmark eye movement effects during natural reading in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 43, 109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huemer, S. V., & Mann, V. (2010). A comprehensive profile of decoding and comprehension in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(4), 485–493.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central coherence theory: linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome: is local coherence impaired? Cognition, 71(2), 149–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Is global coherence impaired? Psychological Medicine, 30(5), 1169–1187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, C. R. G., Happe, F., Golden, H., Marsden, A. J. S., Tregay, J., Simonoff, E., & Charman, T. (2009). Reading and arithmetic in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: peaks and dips in attainment. Neuropsychology, 23(6), 718–728.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, H. S. S. L., Liversedge, S. P., Blythe, H. I., White, S. J., Gathercole, S. E., & Rayner, K. (2008). Children’s and adults’ processing of anomaly and implausibility during reading: Evidence from eye movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(5), 708–723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., Kana, R. K., & Minshew, N. J. (2007). Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: Evidence from an fMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cerebral Cortex, 17(4), 951–961.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 127, 1811–1821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension—A construction integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, A. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2016). lmerTest: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 2.0–30. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest.

  • Liversedge, S. P., & Findlay, J. M. (2000). Saccadic eye movements and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 6–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • López, B., & Leekam, S. R. (2003). Do children with autism fail to process information in context? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(2), 285–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2001). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R., & Norbury, C. F. (2014). Levels of text comprehension in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD): the influence of language phenotype. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(11), 2756–2768.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R., & Norbury, C. F. (2015). Making inferences from text: it’s vocabulary that matters. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(4), 1224–1232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2006). Frequency of reading, math, and writing disabilities in children with clinical disorders. Learning and Individual Differences, 16(2), 145–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1992). Inference during reading. Psychological Review, 99(3), 440–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1998). Autism as a disorder of complex information processing. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 4(2), 129–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1995). Speech and language in high-functioning autistic individuals. Neuropsychology, 9(2), 255–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1997). Neuropsychologic funcitoning in autism: Profile of complex information processing disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3(4), 303–316.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Williams, D. L., & McFadden, K. (2008). Information processing, neural connectivity, and neuronal organization. In A. W. Zimmerman (Ed.), Autism: Current theories and evidence (pp. 381–405). Totowa: Humana Press

  • Nation, K., Clarke, P., Wright, B., & Williams, C. (2006). Patterns of reading ability in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(7), 911–919.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, T. M., Macomber, D., Naples, A. J., Babitz, T., Volkmar, F., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2007). Hyperlexia in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 760–774.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C., & Nation, K. (2011). Understanding variability in reading comprehension in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: interactions with language status and decoding skill. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(3), 191–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C. F. (2005). Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 142–171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: a comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37(3), 227–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-plus. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2016). R: language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1457–1506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Carlson, M., & Frazier, L. (1983). The interaction of syntax and semantics during sentence processing: Eye movements in the analysis of semantically biased sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 358–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Warren, T., Juhasz, B. J., & Liversedge, S. P. (2004). The effect of plausibility on eye movements in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 30(6), 1290–1301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reichle, E. D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2003). The EZ Reader model of eye-movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(04), 445–476.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saldaña, D., Carreiras, M., & Frith, U. (2009). Orthographic and phonological pathways in hyperlexic readers with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(3), 240–253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saldaña, D., & Frith, U. (2007). Do readers with autism make briging inferences from world knowledge? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 310–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sansosti, F. J., Was, C., Rawson, K. A., & Remaklus, B. L. (2013). Eye movements during processing of text requiring bridging inferences in adolescents with higher functioning autism spectrum disorders: A preliminary investigation. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(12), 1535–1542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals, fourth edition (CELF-4). Toronto: The Psychological Corporation/A Harcourt Assessment Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., & Frith, U. (1986). Comprhension in hyperlexic readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42(3), 392–415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M.J., Stothard, S.E., Clarke, P., Bowyer-Crane, C., Harrington, A., Truelove, E., Nation, K. & Hulme, C. (2010). York assessment of reading for comprehension: Passage reading—secondary version. London: GL Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (1981). On the nature of linguistic funcitoning in early infantile Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11(1), 45–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taraban, R., & McClelland, J. L. (1988). Constituent attachment and thematic role assignment in sentence processing: Influences of content based expectations. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 597–632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venables, W. N., & Ripley, B. D. (2002). Modern applied statistics with S (4th edn.). New York, NY: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-21706-2.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, T., & McConnell, K. (2007). Investigating effects of selectional restriction violations and plausibility violation severity on eye-movements in reading. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(4), 770–775.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, T., McConnell, K., & Rayner, K. (2008). Effects of context on eye movements when reading about possible and impossible events. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 34(4), 1001–1010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S., Frith, U., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Swettenham, J., & Ramus, F. (2006). A double dissociation between sensorimotor impairments and reading disability: A comparison of autistic and dyslexic children. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(5), 748–761.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, S. J. (2013). The triple I hypothesis: Taking another (’s) perspective on executive dysfunction in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(1), 114–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2006). Neuropsychologic functioning in children with autism: Further evidence for disordered complex information-processing. Child Neuropsychology, 12(4–5), 279–298.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A., Langston, M. C., & Graesser, A. C. (1995). The contruction of situation models in narrative comprehension—An event-indexing model. Psychological Science, 6(5), 292–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by the Mayflower scholarship, provided by the Faculty of Social, Human and Mathematical Sciences at the University of Southampton, and was completed as part of the first author’s doctoral thesis. We would like to thank all of the volunteers who participated, and Holly Joseph, for allowing us to use the stimuli that she created for Joseph et al. (2008).

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the development of the study rationale, concept, and design. PLH collected the data and completed data analyses. All authors contributed to data interpretation. PLH drafted the manuscript and both VB and SPL have provided feedback and approved the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Philippa L. Howard.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Standards

The University of Southampton’s Ethics and Research Governance Board ethically approved this research and all participants gave informed written consent before taking part.

Electronic supplementary material

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Howard, P.L., Liversedge, S.P. & Benson, V. Investigating the Use of World Knowledge During On-line Comprehension in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 47, 2039–2053 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3129-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3129-x

Keywords

Navigation