Skip to main content

Reading Comprehension and Reading Comprehension Difficulties

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Reading Development and Difficulties

Abstract

The Simple View of Reading indicates that reading comprehension is based upon two broad skills, language comprehension and word reading. This chapter explores the many factors that directly impact language comprehension and reading comprehension apart from word reading skills. Vocabulary, inferencing, background knowledge, comprehension monitoring, and knowledge of text structure are all explored in detail. How these factors interact with reading comprehension and with one other is described, and how to best improve these skills in struggling comprehenders is also presented.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    It should be noted that the usage of the term “tier” by Beck et al. (to denote three levels of vocabulary) is unrelated to the use of that term in the context of Response to Intervention (RTI).

References

  • Anderson, R. C., Stevens, K. C., Shifrin, Z., & Osborn, J. H. (1978). Instantiation of word meanings in children. Journal of Literacy Research, 10(2), 149–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, J. E., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J. (2007). Children’s use of gender and order-of-mention during pronoun comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(4), 527–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Your memory: A user’s guide (3rd ed.). London: Prion Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–90). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. (1984). Spontaneous versus instructed use of multiple standards for evaluating comprehension: Effects of age, reading proficiency, and type of standard. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38(2), 289–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, M. A., & Dennis, M. (1998). Discourse after early-onset hydrocephalus: Core deficits in children of average intelligence. Brain and Language, 61(3), 309–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, M. A., Dennis, M., & Haefele-Kalvaitis, J. (1996). The effects of knowledge availability and knowledge accessibility on coherence and elaborative inferencing in children from six to fifteen years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61(3), 216–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, M. A., Faulkner, H., Wilkinson, M., & Dennis, M. (2004). Meaning construction and integration in children with hydrocephalus. Brain and Language, 89, 47–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barzillai, M., Morris, R., Lovett, M. & Wolf, M. (2010). Poster Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Scientific Studies in Reading.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bast, J., & Reitsma, P. (1998). Analyzing the development of individual differences in terms of Matthew effects in reading: Results from a Dutch longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 34(6), 1373–1399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (2001). Inviting students into the pursuit of meaning. Educational Psychology Review, 13(3), 225–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L. (2005). Choosing words to teach. In A. Hiebert & M. Kamil (Eds.), Teaching and learning vocabulary: Bringing research to practice (pp. 209–222). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I., Perfetti, C., & McKeown, M. (1982). Effects of long-term vocabulary instruction on lexical access and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 506–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Best, R. M., Floyd, R. G., & McNamara, D. S. (2008). Differential competencies contributing to children’s comprehension of narrative and expository texts. Reading Psychology, 29(2), 137–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A. (2005). Size and sequence in vocabulary development: Implications for choosing words for primary grade vocabulary instruction. In A. Hiebert & M. Kamil (Eds.), Teaching and learning vocabulary: Bringing research to practice (pp. 223–242). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A., & Boote, C. (2006). An effective method for building meaning vocabulary in primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 44–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding: Development and disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2001). Uncommon understanding. East Sussex, England: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blewitt, P., Rump, K. M., Shealy, S. E., & Cook, S. A. (2009). Shared book reading: When and how questions affect young children’s word learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2), 294–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., Lahey, M., Hood, L., Lifter, K., & Fiess, K. (1980). Complex sentences: Acquisition of syntactic connectives and the semantic relations they encode. Journal of Child Language, 7(2), 235–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bormuth, J. R. (1967). Cloze readability procedure. CSEIP Occasional Report 1. Los Angeles, CA: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. N., & Kirby, J. R. (2010). Effects of morphological instruction on vocabulary acquisition. Reading and Writing, 23(5), 515–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010). The effects of morphological instruction on literacy skills A systematic review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 144–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S. A. (2011). Efficacy of phonics teaching for reading outcomes: Indications from post NRP research. In S. A. Brady & D. Braze & C. A. Fowler (Eds.), Explaining individual differences in reading. Theory and evidence (pp. 69–96). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 717–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D., & Nitsch, K. E. (1978). Coming to understand things we could not previously understand. In J. F. Kavanaugh & W. Strange (Eds.), Speech and language in the laboratory, school, and clinic (pp. 267–307). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K. (1996). Story knowledge and comprehension skill. In C. Cornoldi & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties: processes and remediation (pp. 167–192). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K. (1999). Ways of reading: How knowledge and use of strategies are related to reading comprehension. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17(2), 293–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K. (2003). Text comprehension and its relation to coherence and cohesion in children’s fictional narratives. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(3), 335–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K. (2006). Individual differences in children’s memory and reading comprehension: An investigation of semantic and inhibitory deficits. Memory, 14, 553–569.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K. (2007). Syntactic awareness and reading ability: Is there any evidence for a special relationship? Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(4), 679–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Nash, H. M. (2011). The influence of connectives on young readers’ processing and comprehension of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(2), 429–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. V. (1999). Inference making and its relation to comprehension failure. Reading and Writing. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11(5–6), 489–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. V. (2006). Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension difficulties. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(4), 683–696.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. V. (2011). Matthew effects in young readers reading comprehension and reading experience aid vocabulary development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44(5), 431–443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2014). Reading comprehension and vocabulary: Is vocabulary more important for some aspects of comprehension? L’Année psychologique, 114, 647–662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. V. (in press). Reading comprehension and vocabulary: Is vocabulary more important for some aspects of comprehension? L’Année Psychologique/Topics in Cognitive Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., Barnes, M. A., & Bryant, P. E. (2001). Comprehension skill, inference making ability and their relation to knowledge. Memory and Cognition, 29(6), 850–859.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., & Bryant, P. E. (2004a). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 671–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., & Lemmon, K. (2004b). Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: the influence of reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 671–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., & Lemmon, K. (2005a). The relation between children’s reading comprehension level and their comprehension of idioms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90(1), 65–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Patson, N., & Andrews, L. (2005b). Age-and ability-related differences in young readers’ use of conjunctions. Journal of Child Language, 32(4), 877–892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Towse, A. S. (2008). To get hold of the wrong end of the stick: Reasons for poor idiom understanding in children with reading comprehension difficulties. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51(6), 1538–1549.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Towse, A. S., & Knight, R. S. (2009). The development of idiom comprehension: An investigation of semantic and contextual processing skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(3), 280–298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carney, J. J., Anderson, D., Blackburn, C., & Blessing, D. (1984). Preteaching vocabulary and the comprehension of social studies materials by elementary school children. Social Education, 48(3), 195–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Compton, D., Tomblin, J. B., & Bridges, M. S. (2012). Prevalence and nature of late-emerging poor readers. Journal of Educational Research, 104(1), 166–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Adlof, S. M. (2005). Developmental changes in reading and reading disabilities. In H. W. Catts & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), The connections between language and reading disabilities (pp. 25–40). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. S., Jacobs, V. A., & Baldwin, L. E. (1990). The reading crisis: Why poor children fall behind. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, L. J. (1983). Reading development and cohesion. Exeter: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charniak, E. (1972). Toward a model of children’s story comprehension (Technical Report 266). Cambridge, MA: Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chien, Y. C., & Wexler, K. (1990). Children’s knowledge of locality conditions in binding as evidence for the modularity of syntax and pragmatics. Language Acquisition, 1(3), 225–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, C. (1969). The acquisition of syntax in children from 5 to 10. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chrysochoou, E., Bablekou, Z., & Tsigilis, N. (2011). Working memory contributions to reading comprehension components in middle childhood children. American Journal of Psychology, 124, 275–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, J. (1999). Every dead thing. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, M. D., McCoach, D. B., & Kapp, S. (2007). Vocabulary intervention for kindergarten students: Comparing extended instruction to embedded instruction and incidental exposure. Learning Disability Quarterly, 30(2), 74–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crosson, A. C., Lesaux, N. K., & Martiniello, M. (2008). Factors that influence comprehension of connectives among language minority children from Spanish-speaking backgrounds. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29(4), 603–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E. (2005). Vocabulary growth through independent reading and reading aloud to children. In E. H. Hiebert & M. L. Kamhi (Eds.), Teaching and learning vocabulary: Bringing research to practice (pp. 45–68). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Currie, N. K., & Cain, K. (2015). Children’s inference generation: The role of vocabulary and working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 137, 57–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, L. E., & Scarborough, H. S. (2006). Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(3), 277–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(4), 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, F. B. (1944). Fundamental factors of comprehension in reading. Psychometrika, 9(3), 185–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, F. B. (1968). Research in comprehension in reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 3(4), 499–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Sousa, I., & Oakhill, J. V. (1996). Do levels of interest have an effect on children’s comprehension monitoring performance? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 66(4), 471–482.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2002). Effects of phonological abilities and linguistic comprehension on the development of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6(1), 51–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(1), 17–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/H0046671.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duke, N. K. (2000). 3.6 minutes per day: The scarcity of informational texts in first grade. Reading Research Quarterly, 35(2), 202–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Pintilie, D. (1992). British picture vocabulary scale. London: NFER-Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich, M. F., Rémond, M., & Tardieu, H. (1999). Processing of anaphoric devices in young skilled and less skilled comprehenders: Differences in metacognitive monitoring. Reading and Writing, 11(1), 29–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., & Arnbak, E. (2002). Components of reading comprehension as predictors of educational achievement. In E. Hjelmquist & C. von Euler (Eds.), Dyslexia and literacy (pp. 69–83). London: Whurr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., & Buch-Iversen, I. (2013). Activation of background knowledge for inference making: Effects on reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(6), 435–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., & Knudsen, L. (2010). The importance of genre knowledge for text comprehension. Insights from a training study. Paper presented at the Annual meeting of the Society for Text and Discourse, August 16–18. Chicago, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Englert, C. S., & Hiebert, E. F. (1984). Children’s developing awareness of text structures in expository materials. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(1), 65–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Englert, C. S., & Thomas, C. C. (1987). Sensitivity to text structure in reading and writing: A comparison between learning-disabled and non-learning disabled students. Learning Disability Quarterly, 10(2), 93–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Florit, E., Roch, M., & Levorato, M. C. (2011). Listening text comprehension of explicit and implicit information in preschoolers: The role of verbal and inferential skills. Discourse Processes, 48(2), 119–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallini, J., Spires, H., Terry, S., & Gleaton, J. (1993). The influence of macro and micro-level cognitive strategies training on text learning. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 26(3), 164–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gambrell, L. B., & Bales, R. J. (1986). Mental imagery and the comprehension-monitoring performance of fourth- and fifth-grade poor readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(4), 454–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gellert, A. S., & Elbro, C. (2013). Cloze tests may be quick, but are they dirty? Development and preliminary validation of a Cloze test of reading comprehension. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 31(1), 16–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., & Hargreaves, D. J. (1988). Accessing sentence participants: The advantage of first mention. Journal of Memory and Language, 27(6), 699–717.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. M., Varner, K. R., & Faust, M. E. (1990). Investigating individual differences in general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(3), 430–445.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Ryan, E. B. (1985). Use of conjunctions in expository texts by skilled and less skilled readers. Journal of Reading Behavior, 17(4), 331–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Learning to read: An unnatural act. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30(1), 179–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., Hoover, W. A., & Peterson, C. L. (1996). Some observations on a simple view of reading. In C. Cornoldi & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties. Processes and intervention (pp. 1–13). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, K. M., Markham, J. C., & Culatta, B. (2005). The development of the Early Expository Comprehension Assessment (EECA): A look at reliability. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 26(4), 195–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, J. W., & Pulver, C. J. (1984). Effects of explicitness, clause order, and reversibility on children’s comprehension of causal relationships. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 399–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, M. l. (1976). Strategies of comprehension of personal pronouns among young children. Enfance, 4–5, 447–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, M., & Weissenborn, J. (1991). Connectives: Developmental issues. In G. Piéraut-le-Bonniec & M. Dolitsky (Eds.), Language bases … discourse bases: Some aspects of contemporary French-language psycholinguistics research (pp. 125–142). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kameenui, E. J., Carnine, D. W., & Freschi, R. (1982). Effects of text construction and instructional procedures for teaching word meanings on comprehension and recall. Reading Research Quarterly, 367–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, J. M., & Betjemann, R. S. (2006). Comprehending the gray oral reading test without reading it: Why comprehension tests should not include passage-independent items. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(4), 368–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R. S., & Olson, R. K. (2008). Reading comprehension tests vary in the skills they assess: Differential dependence on decoding and oral comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12(3), 281–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendeou, P., Bohn-Gettler, C., White, M. J., & van Den Broek, P. (2008). Children’s inference generation across different media. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(3), 259–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendeou, P., Lynch, J. S., van den Broek, P., Espin, C., White, M., & Kremer, K. E. (2005). Developing successful readers: Building early narrative comprehension skills through television viewing and listening. Early Childhood Education Journal, 33, 91–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M., & Lynch, J. S. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: The independent contributions of oral language and decoding skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 765–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Language and Reading Research Consortium. (2015). The dimensionality of language ability in young children. Child Development, 86, 1948–1965. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12450.

  • Leach, J. M., Scarborough, H. S., & Rescorla, L. (2003). Late-emerging reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 211–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepola, J., Lynch, J., Laakkonen, E., Silvén, M., & Niemi, P. (2012). The role of inference making and other language skills in the development of narrative listening comprehension in 4–6-year-old children. Reading Research Quarterly, 47(3), 259–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levorato, M. C., Nesi, B., & Cacciari, C. (2004). Reading comprehension and understanding idiomatic expressions: A developmental study. Brain and Language, 91(3), 303–314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levorato, M. C., Roch, M., & Nesi, B. (2007). A longitudinal study of idiom and text comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 34(3), 473–494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, J. S., & van den Broek, P. (2007). Understanding the glue of narrative structure: Children’s on- and off-line inferences about characters’ goals. Cognitive Development, 22(3), 323–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, J. S., van den Broek, P., Kremer, K., Kendeou, P., White, M. J., & Lorch, E. P. (2008). The development of narrative comprehension and its relation to other early reading skills. Reading Psychology, 29(4), 327–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, N. S. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9(1), 111–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M. (1979). Realizing that you don’t understand: Elementary school children’s awareness of inconsistencies. Child Development, 50, 643–655.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you?. New York: Penguin Putnam Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • McMaster, K. L., van den Broek, P., Espin, C. A., White, M. J., Rapp, D. N., Kendeou, P., et al. (2012). Making the right connections: Differential effects of reading intervention for subgroups of comprehenders. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(1), 100–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Learning from texts: Effects of prior knowledge and text coherence. Discourse Processes, 22, 247–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Medo, M. A., & Ryder, R. J. (1993). The effects of vocabulary instruction on readers’ ability to make causal connections. Literacy Research and Instruction, 33(2), 119–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Megherbi, H., & Ehrlich, M. F. (2005). Language impairment in less skilled comprehenders: The on-line processing of anaphoric pronouns in a listening situation. Reading and Writing, 18(7–9), 715–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mehegan, C., & Dreifuss, F. E. (1972). Hyperlexia. Neurology, 22(11), 1105–1111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A met analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49, 270–291. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B. J., & Freedle, R. O. (1984). Effects of discourse type on recall. American Educational Research Journal, 21(1), 121–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 665–681.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English? Reading Research Quarterly, 19(3), 304–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., & Herman, P. A. (1987). Breadth and depth of vocabulary knowledge: Implications for acquisition and instruction. In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 19–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., & Scott, J. (2000). Vocabulary processes. In M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. III, pp. 269–284). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K. (1999). Reading skills in hyperlexia: A developmental perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 338–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Cocksey, J., Taylor, J. S., & Bishop, D. V. (2010). A longitudinal investigation of early reading and language skills in children with poor reading comprehension. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(9), 1031–1039.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Norbury, C. F. (2005). Why reading comprehension fails: Insights from developmental disorders. Topics in Language Disorders, 25(1), 21–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1998). Semantic processing and the development of word-recognition skills: Evidence from children with reading comprehension difficulties. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(1), 85–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Washington, DC: The National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/nrp/smallbook.htm).

  • Neale, M. D. (1997). The Neale analysis of reading ability—Revised (NARA–II). Windsor, England: NFER-Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V. (1984). Constructive processes in skilled and less-skilled comprehenders’ memory for sentences. British Journal of Psychology, 73(1), 13–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. (1984). Inferential and memory skills in children’s comprehension of stories. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 31–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.1984.tb00842.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(2), 91–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2017). Children with specific text comprehension problems. In K. Cain, D. Compton, & R. Parrila (Eds.), Theories of reading development. John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(4), 443–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J., Cain, K., & Elbro, C. (2014). Understanding and teaching reading comprehension. A handbook. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., & McCarthy, D. (2015). Inference processing in children: the contributions of depth and breadth of vocabulary knowledge. In E. O’Brien, A. Cook & R. Lorch (Eds.), Inferences during reading, Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., McCarthy, D., & Field, Z. (2012). Making the link between vocabulary knowledge and comprehension skill. In A. Britt, S. Goldman, & J.-F. Rouet (Eds.), From words to reading for understanding (p. 101). Hoboken, NJ: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V., & Yuill, N. (1986). Pronoun resolution in skilled and less-skilled comprehenders: Effects of memory load and inferential complexity. Language and Speech, 29(1), 25–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V., Yuill, N., & Donaldson, M. L. (1990). Understanding of causal expressions in skilled and less skilled text comprehenders. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8(4), 401–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V., Yuill, N. M., & Parkin, A. (1986). On the nature of the difference between skilled and less-skilled comprehenders. Journal of Research in Reading, 9, 80–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V., Hartt, J., & Samols, D. (2005). Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Reading and Writing, 18, 657–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 554–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palinscar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1(2), 117–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pappas, C. C. (1993). Is narrative ‘primary’? Some insights from kindergarteners’ pretend readings of stories and information books. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25(1), 97–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S. G. (2005). Reinterpreting the development of reading skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 40(2), 184–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paris, A. H., & Paris, S. G. (2003). Assessing narrative comprehension in young children. Reading Research Quarterly, 38(1), 36–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paris, A. H., & Paris, S. G. (2007). Teaching narrative comprehension strategies to first graders. Cognition and Instruction, 25(1), 1–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S. G., & Jacobs, J. E. (1984). The benefits of informed instruction for children’s reading awareness and comprehension skills. Child Development, 55(6), 2083–2093.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357–383. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430701530730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. (1986). Semantic and pragmatic uses of ‘but’. Journal of Child Language, 13(3), 583–590.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1987). The connective ‘and’: Do older children use it less as they learn other connectives. Journal of Child Language, 14(2), 375–381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pichert, J. W., & Anderson, R. C. (1977). Taking different perspectives on a story. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 309–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, M. M., Barnes, M. A., & Barron, R. W. (2010). The role of illustrations in children’s inferential comprehension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(3), 243–255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. The new science of language and mind. London: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, G. M. (1976). Mental imagery helps eight-year-olds remember what they read. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68(3), 355–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: The nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Sideridis, G. D., Mouzaki, A., & Simos, P. G. (2007). Development of lexical mediation in the relation between reading comprehension and word reading skills in Greek. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(3), 165–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pyykkonen, P., & Jarvikivi, J. (2012). Children and situation models of multiple events. Developmental Psychology, 48, 521–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, J. (1972). Children’s comprehension of syntactic features found in some extension readers. Occasional paper: Centre for Research in Educational Sciences, University of Edinburgh.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 21(4), 803–814. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenshine, B., Meister, C., & Chapman, S. (1996). Teaching students to generate questions: A review of the intervention studies. Review of Educational Research, 66(2), 181–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sachs, J. S. (1967). Recognition memory for syntactic and semantic aspects of connected discourse. Perception and Psychophysics, 2(9), 437–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy (pp. 97–110). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seigneuric, A., & Ehrlich, M. F. (2005). Contribution of working memory capacity to children’s reading comprehension: A longitudinal investigation. Reading and Writing, 18(7–9), 617–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., & Elder, L. (1986). Beginning reading without phonology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3(1), 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, T., Kamil, M. L., & Tobin, A. W. (1982). Cloze as a measure of intersentential comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 17(2), 229–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, B. K., & Hudson, J. A. (1997). Coherence and cohesion in children’s stories. In J. Costermans & M. Fayol (Eds.), Processing interclausal relationships: Studies in the production and comprehension of text (pp. 23–48). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skarakis-Doyle, E. (2002). Young children’s detection of violations in familiar stories and emerging comprehension monitoring. Discourse Processes, 33(2), 175–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silva, M. T., & Cain, K. (2015). The relations between lower- and higher-level oral language skills and their role in prediction of early reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 321–331. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., & Swinney, D. A. (1992). The role of schemas in reading text: A real-time examination. Discourse Processes, 15(3), 303–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., & Frith, U. (1986). Comprehension in “hyperlexic” readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42(3), 392–415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Learning to read with a language impairment. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 397–412). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Song, H. J., & Fisher, C. (2005). Who’s “she”? Discourse prominence influences preschoolers’ comprehension of pronouns. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(1), 29–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, E., & Deacon, S. H. (2013). Morphological awareness and vocabulary acquisition: A longitudinal examination of their relationship in English-speaking children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spooren, W., & Sanders, T. (2008). The acquisition order of coherence relations: On cognitive complexity in discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 40(12), 2003–2026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spörer, N., Brunstein, J. C., & Kieschke, U. (2009). Improving students’ reading comprehension skills: Effects of strategy instruction and reciprocal teaching. Learning and Instruction, 19(3), 272–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, S. A., & Fairbanks, M. M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56(1), 72–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(4), 360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1982). Children’s concept of time: The development of story schema. In W. Friedman, J. (Ed.), The developmental psychology of time (pp. 255–282). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stetter, M. E., & Hughes, M. T. (2010). Using story grammar to assist students with learning disabilities and reading difficulties improve their comprehension. Education & Treatment of Children, 33(1), 115–151. https://doi.org/10.1353/etc.0.0087.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, B. M., & Samuels, S. J. (1983). Children’s use of text structure in the recall of expository material. American Educational Research Journal, 20(4), 517–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tannenbaum, K. R., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (2006). Relationships between word knowledge and reading comprehension in third-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(4), 381–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, R. L. (1973). Reading as reasoning. Reading Research Quarterly, 9(2), 135–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomesen, M., & Aarnoutse, C. (1998). Effects of an instructional programme for deriving word meanings 1. Educational Studies, 24(1), 107–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tompkins, V., Guo, Y., & Justice, L. M. (2013). Inference generation, story comprehension, and language skills in the preschool years. Reading & Writing, 26(3), 403–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tong, X., Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., Cain, K., & Parrila, R. (2011). Morphological awareness: A key to understanding poor reading comprehension in English. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(3), 523–534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., Nesdale, A. R., & Pratt, C. (1983). The development of young children’s awareness of logical inconsistencies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36(1), 97–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, L. K. (1983). The development of discourse mapping processes: The on-line interpretation of anaphoric expressions. Cognition, 13(3), 309–341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P. W. (1997). Discovering the cement of the universe: The development of event comprehension from childhood to adulthood. In P. W. van den Broek, P. J. Bauer, & T. Bourg (Eds.), Developmental spans in event comprehension and representation (pp. 321–342). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P. W., Lorch, R. F., Linderholm, T., & Gustafson, M. (2001). The effects of readers’ goals on inference generation and memory for text. Memory and Cognition, 29(8), 1081–1087.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, J., Lorch, E. P., & Thurlow, R. (1996). Children’s and adults memory for television stories: The role of causal factors, story-grammar categories, and hierarchical structure. Child Development, 67(6), 3010–3028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Venezky, R. (2000). The origins of the present-day chasms between adult literacy needs and school literacy instruction. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4(1), 19–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walberg, H. J., Strykowski, B. F., Rovai, E., & Hung, S. S. (1984). Exceptional performance. Review of Educational Research, 54(1), 87–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S., Hamilton, S., Oakhill, J. V., & Holliday, R. E. (2008). False recollection in children with reading comprehension difficulties. Cognition, 106(1), 222–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whaley, J. F. (1981). Readers’ expectations for story structure. Reading Research Quarterly, 17(1), 90–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. P., Nubla-Kung, A. M., Pollini, S., Stafford, K. B., Garcia, A., & Snyder, A. E. (2007). Teaching cause–effect text structure through social studies content to at-risk second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(2), 111–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. P., Stafford, K. B., Lauer, K. D., Hall, K. M., & Pollini, S. (2009). Embedding reading comprehension training in content-area instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(1), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuill, N. M., & Oakhill, J. V. (1988). Effects of inference awareness training on poor reading comprehension. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2(1), 33–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuill, N. M., & Oakhill, J. V. (1991). Children’s problems in text comprehension: An experimental investigation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yuill, N. M., Oakhill, J. V., & Parkin, A. (1989). Working memory, comprehension ability and the resolution of text anomaly. British Journal of Psychology, 80(3), 351–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A. (1994). Effect of genre expectations on text comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 920–933.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jane Oakhill .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Oakhill, J., Cain, K., Elbro, C. (2019). Reading Comprehension and Reading Comprehension Difficulties. In: Kilpatrick, D., Joshi, R., Wagner, R. (eds) Reading Development and Difficulties. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-26550-2_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics