Introduction

Sperm competition has been defined as “the competition within a single female between the sperm from two or more males for the fertilisation of the ova” (Parker 1970). Thus, the presence of sperm from more than one male within a female is required, but this does not necessarily imply skewed sperm utilisation patterns (Simmons and Siva-Jothy 1998). Although studies on insects and birds predominate, sperm competition is currently accepted as ubiquitous in animals (reviewed in Smith 1984; Birkhead and Møller 1998; Simmons 2001) and has pollen competition as a parallel in plants (Delph and Havens 1998). The simultaneous presence of sperm or pollen from different donors also allows for cryptic female choice, which is notoriously difficult to disentangle from sperm competition (e.g. Eberhard 1996).

Despite the fact that many hermaphroditic animals have specialised sperm storage and digestion structures that enhance sperm competition and cryptic sperm choice (reviewed in Baur 1998; Michiels 1998), surprisingly few studies have investigated these processes in detail. Moreover, those studies that did mainly involved reciprocally-mating simultaneous hermaphrodites (e.g. Bojat et al. 2001; Rogers and Chase 2002; Beese et al. 2006). Even in those species the details of sperm storage, removal and digestion are only partly understood. Yet, understanding the details of the fate of received sperm (hereafter called allosperm) is pivotal for explaining how sexual selection, via sperm competition and cryptic sperm choice, shapes the evolution of the male and female reproductive morphology and physiology (see also Schärer et al. 2007).

The details of allosperm processing by a recipient may also be essential for understanding another life history trait of many simultaneous hermaphrodites, the mating system. Hermaphroditic animals are often able to self-fertilise as well as cross-fertilise, and sperm recipients may thus exert considerable control over which auto- and allosperm are used for fertilisation. There are many examples of populations showing mixed mating in the field, i.e. producing selfed or outcrossed offspring (e.g. Henry et al. 2005; Jokela et al. 2006). A few studies showed that individuals can also perform mixed fertilisation, i.e. producing selfed and outcrossed eggs within one egg mass (e.g. Cain 1956; Städler et al. 1993; Gow et al. 2005). There are theoretical considerations as to whether and when producing selfed and/or outcrossed offspring is a good strategy to adopt (Jarne et al. 1993; Jarne and Charlesworth 1993). Most importantly, outcrossing is generally predicted to be favoured when inbreeding depression is high and individual heterozygosity is important. Selfing is predicted to predominate when there are high costs for the male function or for mate searching, even though selfing may lead to a loss of heterozygosity (e.g. Jarne and Städler 1995; Jokela et al. 2006). Overall, the higher efficiency of gene transmission through selfing compared to outcrossing is thought to be counterbalanced by inbreeding depression (e.g. Meunier et al. 2004).

The abovementioned consequences of selfing and outcrossing usually constitute the basis for the hypotheses devised to explain the evolution of mixed-mating systems. Surprisingly, all of this seems to be done without consideration of the underlying mechanisms involved in allosperm receipt. For example, is the production of selfed and outcrossed eggs a passive process or actively controlled by the female function? Moreover, often the occurrence of selfed and outcrossed eggs within one egg batch has not been considered. Such issues probably remained unaddressed because of the predominance of experimental and theoretical investigations of such mixed-mating systems in plants. Clearly, there are essential differences. For example, plants do not copulate and can therefore often not avoid fertilisation by own pollen (they are at the mercy of pollination). But, perhaps most importantly for the case considered here, they cannot store pollen the way that animals can store allosperm; animals can often do so for a whole breeding season or even much longer. This results in essential differences between pollen competition and sperm competition that cannot be ignored when looking for explanations for the occurrence of the production of selfed and outcrossed offspring. Hence, in order to explain how hermaphroditic animals optimise offspring production through their female function, via sperm competition, cryptic sperm choice and mixed fertilisation, detailed knowledge about the allosperm storage process will be essential.

Upon closer inspection, there seems to be a near-complete lack of knowledge about how the production of mixed offspring might work mechanistically in hermaphroditic animals. Despite the many efforts to explain mixed mating in evolutionary terms, very few studies have delved into the functional aspects of such a system, even though this was already advocated more than a decade ago (Jarne et al. 1993). As a consequence, there is detailed knowledge about the behavioural aspects of sperm transfer and the outcome of insemination and sperm competition (in terms of paternity and mixed mating). But, surprisingly little is known about the processes that take place in between, that is, the mechanisms that determine which sperm are used for the fertilisation of eggs and whether this results in mixed fertilisation.

To address the above issues, we have looked at aspects of sperm digestion, storage and use in a unilaterally inseminating hermaphrodite that can self- and cross-fertilise, the great pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis (Pulmonata, Basommatophora, Lymnaeidae). This species lays eggs in masses that have been reported to contain both selfed and outcrossed offspring (Cain 1956). Although these snails are simultaneous hermaphrodites, they can only mate in one sexual role within one copulation (Koene and Ter Maat 2005). Received sperm can either be digested in the bursa copulatrix (e.g. Horstmann 1955; De Jong-Brink 1969; Rogers and Reeder 1987; Clelland et al. 2001) or be stored for fertilising eggs (Cain 1956). In Basommatophora, sperm storage allegedly occurs at the junction where the hermaphroditic duct divides into a male and female tract, called the carrefour region, and where the fertilisation pouch is located (De Jong-Brink 1984; Geraerts and Joosse 1984). We here investigated the functional aspects involved in the processing of allosperm in this species.

Material and methods

Animals

Specimens of the great pond snail L. stagnalis (L.) were obtained from our laboratory culture. For the experiments we started out with immature, virgin individuals which had a shell height that was <17 mm. These were all kept individually in perforated plastic jars in the same large tank with running low-copper water of 20°C. They were each fed one disc of lettuce (19.6 cm2) per day.

Microsatellites

For the initial tests of microsatellite heterozygosity we randomly selected mature specimens from the breeding tanks. Tissue samples were taken from the foot for which animals were anaesthetised using approximately 2 ml of 50 mM MgCl2. The experimental animals were then returned to their individual housing and left to recover. The tissue samples were frozen at −80°C. We genotyped the individuals for 12 different microsatellite loci. For this, the tissue was thawed on ice and total DNA was extracted and purified using the Wizard SV Genomic DNA purification system (Promega, Madison, WI, USA): 150 μl of Nuclei Lysis Buffer and 2 μl of proteinase K were added to each sample. After vortexing, the samples were incubated for 2–3 h at 60°C. After grinding up the tissue with a pestle, 250 μl of SV RNA Lysis buffer was mixed in. This was followed by centrifuging for 5 min at 14,000 rpm. The DNA-containing supernatant was purified using a spin column by washing with SV RNA Lysis buffer and 500 μl wash buffer (3×), each time spinning off for 30 s at 14,000 rpm (with a final dry spin). The DNA was then dissolved by adding 100 μl nuclease-free water and, after 5 min, spun off for 1 min at 800 rpm. The dissolved DNA was stored at −20°C.

The microsatellites were amplified via PCR, using nine published primers (Knott et al. 2003; Accession codes AY225955–AY225963) and three unpublished primers (kindly provided by E. Knott and M. Puurtinen). It is important to note here that in the Knott et al. (2003) publication the reverse primers are displayed in 3′–5′ direction rather than the conventional 5′–3′ order. Microsatellites were amplified in 29 μl reactions containing 10× PCR buffer (MRC Holland), 0.17 mM dNTPs (Invitrogen), 0.125 U Taq Polymerase (MRC Holland), 0.17 mM of each primer, 5 μl of the dissolved DNA, and distilled water. The following cycling profile was used: denaturation for 2 min at 94°C, followed by 35 cycles of 40 s at 92°C, 40 s at 55°C and 30 s at 70°C, and a final extension period of 1.5 min at 72°C. To assess polymorphism we separated 14 μl of each PCR product on Spreadex EL 600 gels with a 100 bp size marker (Promega) on each gel and stained the gels with SYBR Gold (Molecular Probes). In the animals from our laboratory culture we found three different alleles for microsatellite loci A16 (AY225956) and B4 (AY225959), while the other loci were monomorphic or had only two alleles. We chose to use only A16 and B4 for the subsequent paternity experiments.

Paternity

After having identified the microsatellite alleles of all the animals, we made combinations of individuals so that in subsequent paternity testing we could unequivocally attribute offspring to either of the parents based on either microsatellite A16 or B4.

We allowed focal animals to copulate with one partner (singly-inseminated) or two partners (doubly-inseminated). All matings were observed, during 8 h on the same day. As soon as these animals had received sperm from their partner, they were returned to their original jar from which any self-fertilised egg masses had been removed. One randomly chosen subset of animals was allowed to mate a second time with a different partner on the following day. Again, all matings were observed and as soon as these animals had received sperm, they were returned to their original jar.

During the following days, we verified daily for egg laying by the singly- and doubly-inseminated animals (resp. N = 5 and N = 8). The first egg mass that was laid was collected and left to hatch in a separate closed container. After three weeks, when the hatchlings were approximately 2 mm, we randomly selected 10 of the hatchlings from each egg mass and stored these separately at −80°C. Once all the animals had produced an egg mass, the hatchlings were thawed and DNA was extracted, using the whole animal, for genotyping loci A16 and B4.

Location of sperm

To find out what happens with the received sperm in the hours following insemination, we paired 96 virgin animals and separated them immediately after the first unilateral insemination. From these 48 pairs, 33 sperm recipients were used for weight measurements and the remaining 15 for histological processing. For both procedures, the animals were randomly used at 0, 3, 6, 24 and 48 h post-copulation. As controls we used respectively seven and three unmated virgin animals. For the weight measurement we first measured the shell height with callipers, and determined the body weight of the animal. We removed the shell and carefully dissected out the bursa copulatrix. We then determined shell weight, wet weight, and bursa copulatrix weight. We used seven unmated virgins for comparison.

For histology, the complete female reproductive tract was dissected out and three unmated virgins were used for comparison. The gonadal tissue was fixed using Bouin fixative. After fixation, the tissue was embedded in paraffin. The serial sections of 7 μm were deparafinated using xylene. The sections were then alternatingly stained with the standard HE method (haematoxylin and eosin) and overnight with Vectashield mounting medium with DAPI (Brunschwig Chemie H1200). The latter staining was used to visualise the highly-condensed DNA in the sperm heads. A naïve observer scored the presence of sperm in (the allosperm groove running along) the different female organs.

Results

Microsatellite variability

Because we used cultured animals for this study, we first determined allelic diversity and heterozygosity for 12 microsatellites. This screening revealed that two of these loci, A16 and B4 were variable enough for further use in paternity testing. Table 1 shows the number of alleles found for each of the loci as well as the expected and observed heterozygosities. For C4 the expected and observed frequencies differed significantly (Pearson χ 2 = 17.55, P < 0.001), and this locus thus deviated from the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium. Figure 1 illustrates the distribution of the different genotypes for loci A16 and B4.

Table 1 Microsatellites screened for variability in the L. stagnali s population from the breeding facility
Fig. 1
figure 1

Genotype distributions of microsatellite loci A16 and B4. The graphs show the frequencies of the different genotypes that were observed for A16 (N = 240) and B4 (N = 115). The alleles of each locus are labelled A, B and C according to their length, from shortest to longest

Paternity analyses

Because L. stagnalis can self-fertilise, we first tested whether they use the received sperm to fertilise their eggs. For the five virgins investigated, we found for each virgin that the 10 offspring analysed from their first egg mass after copulation contained only outcrossed eggs. Hence, although we only genotyped a proportion of each egg mass, the animals seemed to switch to complete outcrossing once sperm had been received. After having established this, we looked at patterns of paternity in focal virgins that copulated with two different partners (doubly-inseminated). Figure 2 shows the distribution of the P 2-values, i.e. the proportion of the offspring fathered by the second sperm donor. This revealed that in general sperm from both donors were used for fertilisation, with an average P 2-value of 0.6 (±0.28 SD; range 0.1–1.0). The second donor’s paternity did not differ significantly from 0.5 (z-test: z 7 = 1.01, P = 0.31).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Sperm of different donors fertilise eggs. Frequency distribution of the P 2-values, i.e. the proportion fathered by the second sperm donor, is shown for hatchlings from the first egg mass after a double insemination. The average P 2-value of 0.6 does not differ significantly from 0.5; the dotted line indicates the expected P 2 distribution if sperm mix randomly (see Results). Below the graph, the paternity of the offspring of each individual mother is ranked from high to low P 2-values. The grey bars represents the proportion fertilised by the second (2nd) father, which is also the P 2-value indicated on the x-axis. The white bars represent the proportion fathered by the first (1st) father, P 1, which equals 1−P 2. N indicates the number of offspring analysed per mother

Location of sperm

The average shell length at the end of the experiment of the animals was 3.06 ± 0.08 cm and did not differ between the animals from the different time intervals (ANOVA: F 5,34 = 1.209, P = 0.326). There was also no difference in body weight (2.75 ± 0.31 g; ANOVA: F 5,34 = 1.844, P = 0.130), shell weight (0.41 ± 0.06 g; ANOVA: F 5,34 = 1.991, P = 0.105) and wet weight (1.21 ± 0.13 g; ANOVA: F 5,34 = 1.718, P = 0.157). The weight of the bursa copulatrix differed significantly among time intervals after copulation (ANOVA: F 5,34 = 26.79, P < 0.0001; Fig. 3). This difference was entirely due to the 3 h group (post hoc Tukey: P < 0.05). Thus, after 3 h most sperm were located in the bursa copulatrix.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Sperm digestion in the bursa copulatrix. The weights of the bursa copulatrix from unmated virgins and virgins at different times after one unilateral sperm receipt are shown. The box plots indicate median, 25th and 75th quartile, and range. Significant differences are indicated with different letters

Table 2 shows the organs within the female reproductive tract where fluorescently-labelled sperm were found (in the allosperm groove). As indicated by the grey bars, (auto)sperm were always present in the hermaphroditic part of the reproductive tract (i.e. ovotestis, seminal vesicles and hermaphroditic duct), also in unmated virgin snails. Immediately after copulation, the received sperm were present in the vaginal duct and the uterus duct leading up to the oothecal gland, which is consistent with previous findings (Loose and Koene 2008). At 3 h post copulation, sperm could be seen throughout the female tract, except for the vaginal duct. This was also the time when sperm were found in the fertilisation pouch. We also found this in one animal that was fixed after 6 h (as indicated by the lighter grey in Table 2). In the female tract of animals that were fixed after more than 6 h, sperm were only found in the bursa copulatrix (besides the hermaphroditic parts).

Table 2 Presence of sperm in the female reproductive tract of L. stagnalis at different intervals after copulation

Discussion

There seems to be a gap in our basic knowledge about sperm receipt in hermaphrodites that is essential for the understanding of sperm competition, cryptic sperm choice, and mixed mating. We have here looked at the fate of received sperm after transfer in the unilaterally-inseminating hermaphroditic pond snail L. stagnalis. We find that receipt of a single ejaculate by a virgin snail results in the first egg mass being completely composed of outcrossed offspring. More importantly, sperm of two donors are stored and both fertilise eggs within an egg mass, which results in a roughly equal share of paternity with rather large variance.

Thus, although the majority of the sperm will be digested (see below), some sperm do get stored. As a result, virgin animals switch to complete outcrossing right after sperm receipt. This is in stark contrast with the mix of selfed and outcrossed offspring that is usually found in population studies (e.g. Puurtinen et al. 2004). To properly understand such patterns, in a future study we plan to investigate allosperm use in subsequent egg masses. The result of the double matings does, however, reveal something essential about how sperm storage works in these animals. Namely, we find that both donors fathered offspring despite a 24 h inter-mating interval. Besides confirming that there is opportunity for sperm competition and cryptic sperm choice, the sperm precedence pattern strongly suggests that sperm of the two donors are well mixed before fertilisation (Harvey and Parker 2000). It has been argued that at such intermediate P 2-values sperm competition will be at its strongest, while highly skewed sperm utilisation patterns can indicate, for instance, non-random sperm mixing or cryptic sperm choice (e.g. Simmons and Siva-Jothy 1998; Simmons 2001).

Our paternity data show that received sperm of two partners are both used and stored. The latter confirms previous observations (e.g. Cain 1956). Our histological data and bursa copulatrix weights indicate that within 6 h the received sperm have either reached the sperm storage site or the bursa copulatrix. Not unexpectedly, we find that received excess sperm get transported into the bursa copulatrix, the gametolytic organ, although initially received sperm were found throughout the female tract. The bulk of the sperm already reaches the bursa copulatrix within the first 3 h after copulation. And within the following hours, the digestive process greatly reduced its volume, as becomes apparent from the weight measurements. If the weight is representative of the digestive process, this would suggest that most of the sperm are digested within 6 h after the end of insemination. This clearly limits the time window within which sperm can reach the site of storage. Moreover, the quick reduction in weight also argues against a possible inhibition of remating via stretch receptors in the bursa copulatrix. This was previously suggested based on the presence of nervous innervation of this organ by a branch of the intestinal nerve (Elo 1938; Koene 2006), which is also important for egg laying behaviour (Ferguson et al. 1993). Although the volume of the bursa copulatrix could be registered via this nerve for some other purpose, it is unlikely that it serves to inhibit remating like it does in the cabbage white butterfly (Pieris rapae crucivora: Sugawara 1979).

It is noteworthy that we find no evidence that sperm are stored in the fertilisation pouch, which is confusingly-enough sometimes also labelled as receptaculum seminis. For L. stagnalis, and Basommatophora in general, sperm storage has long been assumed to occur in the fertilisation pouch (De Jong-Brink 1984; Geraerts and Joosse 1984). However, a closer look at the literature reveals that this suggestion is not based on direct evidence, but rather on an extrapolation from the situation in Stylommatophora where a well-defined allosperm storage organ is attached to their fertilisation pouch (spermathecal tubules; e.g. Duncan 1975). Tomé and Ribeiro (1998) also did not find any evidence for sperm storage in the carrefour region of the freshwater snail Biomphalaria tenagophila. Moreover, previous work on L. stagnalis also indicates that the fertilisation pouch is not suited for allosperm storage but the most likely site where fertilisation can ultimately take place before egg packaging starts (Plesch et al. 1971).

Allosperm cannot be stored downstream from the fertilisation pouch, because past that point the female glands are all involved in provisioning and packaging of the already-fertilised eggs. Hence, the three possible upstream sites for allosperm storage are the ovotestis, seminal vesicles and hermaphroditic duct. The ovotestis, where the own sperm and eggs are produced, has been put forward by two studies using radiolabelled sperm (Lobato Paraense 1976; Monteiro and Kawano 2000). However, given the time interval between copulation and detection of the label that those studies used (resp. 5 days and 6 h to 41 days) and the rapid sperm digestion that we find (within 6 h), this idea requires re-evaluation. The seminal vesicles have not been proposed before, even though they store the ripe autosperm and are thus specialised for this function. Of course, storage in these vesicles would complicate the separation of auto- and allosperm. Since there is no indication for separate compartments here, this would indicate that the priority of allosperm in the fertilisation process must be due to a difference in activation and/or capacitation of auto- and allosperm. The hermaphroditic duct has been suggested for allosperm storage by Larambergue (1939) and Bretschneider (1948).

In sum, although Cain already showed in 1956 that sperm can be stored and used for fertilisation for a long time in this species, the precise site of sperm storage remains elusive. Based on the foregoing and the data presented here, we can now narrow the potential storage sites down to the hermaphroditic duct, the seminal vesicles, and possibly the ovotestis. With the method we used here, we could unfortunately not distinguish autosperm from allosperm, so clearly this calls for an approach in which the sperm can be traced back individually after transfer (which seems now available: Schärer et al. 2004, 2007).

Revealing the exact site of allosperm storage will clearly provide novel insights into how mixed fertilisation and sperm competition work in this species. If allo- and autosperm are stored in the same site—the seminal vesicles or ovotestis—this might suggest that these animals have little control over which sperm fertilise eggs. Although it is of course still possible that these animals can control paternity through cryptic sperm choice, for instance via physiological or biochemical recognition of sperm. Separate storage—in the hermaphroditic duct—would imply more and easier control over selfing and outcrossing, and might best explain the complete outcrossing that we find here after one mating. Evidently, sperm competition, cryptic sperm choice and mixed mating can therefore not be treated as separate issues in these simultaneous hermaphrodites, but might be intricately entangled at the level of sperm receipt and storage.