Skip to main content
Log in

Teaching children how to include the inversion principle in their reasoning about quantitative relations

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The basis of this intervention study is a distinction between numerical calculus and relational calculus. The former refers to numerical calculations and the latter to the analysis of the quantitative relations in mathematical problems. The inverse relation between addition and subtraction is relevant to both kinds of calculus, but so far research on improving children’s understanding and use of the principle of inversion through interventions has only been applied to the solving of a + b − b = ? sums. The main aim of the intervention described in this article was to study the effects of teaching children about the explicit use of inversion as part of the relational calculus needed to solve inverse addition and subtraction problems using a calculator. The study showed that children taught about relational calculus differed significantly from those who were taught numerical procedures, and also that effects of the intervention were stronger when children were taught about relational calculus with mixtures of indirect and direct word problems than when these two types of problem were given to them in separate blocks.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Lai, Baroody and Johnson (2008) report an intervention study in which they taught the children about the inverse relation; however, it was taught as an aim in itself, not as a tool to solve computations. For this reason, it is not reviewed here.

  2. The characters in the story were common cartoon characters at the time. Permission was obtained for use of these in the research. For copyright reasons, a different illustration is used in Fig. 1.

References

  • Baroody, A. J., & Lai, M.-L. (2007). Preschoolers’ understanding of the addition–subtraction inverse principle: A Taiwanese sample. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 9, 131–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beishuizen, M. (1997). Development of mathematical strategies and procedures up to 100. In M. Beishuizen, K. P. E. Gravemeijer, & E. C. D. M. van Lieshout (Eds.), The role of contexts and models in the development of mathematical strategies and procedures (pp. 127–162). Utrecht: Beta.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booth, L. R. (1981). Child-methods in secondary mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 12, 29–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brissiaud, R. (1994). Teaching and development: Solving “missing addend” problems using subtraction. In B. Schneuwly & M. Brossard (Eds.): Learning and development: contributions from Vygotsky. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 9, 343–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P., Christie, C., & Rendu, A. (1999). Children’s understanding of the relation between addition and subtraction: Inversion, identity and decomposition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 194–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P., & Nunes, T. (2009) Key understandings in mathematics learning. Paper 2. Understanding whole numbers. Available from: http://www.nuffieldfoundation.org/fileLibrary/pdf/P2.pdf

  • Carpenter, T. P., & Moser, J. M. (1982). The development of addition and subtraction problem solving. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M. Moser, & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Addition and subtraction: A cognitive perspective (pp. 10–24). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., Kurland, M., & Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33(3), 386–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dowker, A. D. (1997). Young children’s addition estimates. Mathematical Cognition, 3, 141–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Prentice, K., Burch, M., Hamlett, C. L., Owen, R., et al. (2003). Explicitly teaching for transfer: Effects on third-grade students’ mathematical problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 293–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. C. (1986). Teaching children to subtract by counting up. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 17, 172–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. C. (1990). Conceptual structures for multiunit numbers: Implications for learning and teaching multidigit addition, subtraction, and place value. Cognition and Instruction, 7, 343–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. C., & Willis, G. B. (1988). Subtracting by counting up: More evidence. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 19(5), 402–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilmore, C. K. (2006). Investigating children’s understanding of inversion using the missing number paradigm. Cognitive Development, 21, 301–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilmore, C. K., & Papadatou-Pastou, M. (2009). Patterns of individual differences in conceptual understanding and arithmetical skill: A meta-analysis. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 11, 25–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, K., Brown, M., Kerslake, D., Kuchermann, D., & Ruddock, G. (1985). Chelsea diagnostic mathematics tests. Fractions 1. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, A. S., Beishuizen, M., & Treffers, A. (1998). The empty number line in Dutch second grades: Realistic versus gradual program design. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29, 443–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, M.-L., Baroody, A. J., & Johnson, A. R. (2008). Fostering Taiwanese preschoolers’ understanding of the addition–subtraction inverse principle. Cognitive Development, 23, 216–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (1996). Children doing mathematics. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Hallett, D., Bell, D., & Evans, D. (2009). Teaching children about the inverse relation between addition and subtraction. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 11, 61–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Sylva, K., & Barros, R. (2009). Development of maths capabilities and confidence in primary school (No. Research Report DCSF-RR118). London: Department for Children, Schools and Families. Available from: http://publications.dcsf.gov.uk/eOrderingDownload/DCSF-RB118.pdf

  • Nunes, T., Schliemann, A. D., & Carraher, D. W. (1993). Street mathematics and school mathematics. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riley, M., Greeno, J. G., & Heller, J. I. (1983). Development of children’s problem solving ability in arithmetic. In H. Ginsburg (Ed.), The development of mathematical thinking (pp. 153–196). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Stern, E. (1998). Conscious and unconscious strategy discoveries: A microgenetic analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 377–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. G. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P. W. (1993). Quantitative reasoning, complexity, and additive structures. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 3, 165–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torbeyns, J., Smedt, B. D., Stassens, N., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Solving subtraction problems by means of indirect addition. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 11, 79–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vergnaud, G. (1979). The acquisition of arithmetical concepts. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 10, 263–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vergnaud, G. (1982). A classification of cognitive tasks and operations of thought involved in addition and subtraction problems. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M. Moser, & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Addition and subtraction: A cognitive perspective (pp. 60–67). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

The authors are grateful to the Economic and Social Research Centre, Teaching and Learning Research Programme for grant #L139251015 which made this research possible. We are very thankful to the schools, teachers, and children without whose generous participation, no research would be possible.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Terezinha Nunes.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Evans, D. et al. Teaching children how to include the inversion principle in their reasoning about quantitative relations. Educ Stud Math 79, 371–388 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-011-9314-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-011-9314-5

Keywords

Navigation