Skip to main content
Log in

Temperament and Home Environment Characteristics as Predictors of Young Children’s Learning Motivation

  • Published:
Early Childhood Education Journal Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated the relationship between the temperament dimensions, home environment factors, and the two types of young children’s learning motivation. The study aimed to investigate the relative contributions of predictors and identify the predictors of young children’s intrinsic and extrinsic learning motivation in a classroom-based setting. To do this, 296 mothers of 5-year-old Korean children responded to a survey on children’s temperament and home environment. Teachers of the same children rated the children’s intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Five temperament dimensions and two home environment factors were associated with children’s motivation in class. After controlling for family socioeconomic status and children’s gender, children’s level of attentional focusing and the verity of developmental stimulus presented at home made unique contributions to their intrinsic learning motivation in class. However, children’s extrinsic motivation was not predicted by any of the children’s temperament and home environment characteristics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder Co: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amabile, T. M., Hill, K. G., Hennessey, B. A., & Tighe, E. M. (1994). The work preference inventory: Assessing intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,66(5), 950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, R., & Hilton, A. (2010). Statistical analysis in microbiology: Statutes. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L., & Wigfield, A. (1999). Dimensions of children’s motivation for reading and their relations to reading activity and reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly,34(4), 452–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broussard, S. C., & Garrison, M. E. B. (2004). The relationship between classroom motivation and academic achievement in elementary-school-aged children. Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal,33(2), 106–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (2003). Home inventory administration manual: Comprehensive edition. Little Rock: University of Arkansas at Little Rock.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlton, M. P., & Winsler, A. (1998). Fostering intrinsic motivation in early childhood classrooms. Early Childhood Education Journal,25(3), 159–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, F., & Burns, B. M. (2005). Attention in preschoolers: Associations with effortful control and motivation. Child Development,76(1), 247–263. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00842.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, N. (2015). Development and validation of the scale on young children’s preferences for daily activities in early childhood institutes. Korean Journal of Child Education and Care,15(3), 293–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Extrinsic rewards and intrinsic motivation in education: Reconsidered once again. Review of Educational Research,71(1), 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general causality orientations scale: Self-determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality,19(2), 109–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1994). Temperament and the self-organization of personality. Development and Psychopathology,6(4), 653–676.

    Google Scholar 

  • Do, K. H. (2008). Relation among father’s child-rearing involvement, father-child communication style, children’s achievement motivation and school adjustment. Journal of Korean Home Economics Education Association,20(4), 139–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach-avoidance motivation in personality: Approach and avoidance temperaments and goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,82(5), 804.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, A. E. (1990). Academic intrinsic motivation in young elementary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology,82(3), 525–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (1998). Role of cognitively stimulating home environment in children’s academic intrinsic motivation: A longitudinal study. Child Development,69(5), 1448–1460. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06223.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hair, J., Anderson, R., Tatham, R., & Black, W. (1998). Multivariate data analysis (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S., & Jackson, B. K. (1992). Trait vs. nontrait conceptualizations of intrinsic/extrinsic motivational orientation. Motivation and Emotion,16(3), 209–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hennessey, B. A., & Zbikowski, S. (1993). Immunizing children against the negative effects of reward: A further examination of the intrinsic motivation training techniques. Creativity Research Journal,6, 297–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirvonen, R., Torppa, M., Nurmi, J. E., Eklund, K., & Ahonen, T. (2016). Early temperament and age at school entry predict task avoidance in elementary school. Learning and Individual Differences,47, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hokoda, A., & Fincham, F. D. (1995). Origins of children’s helpless and mastery achievement patterns in the family. Journal of Educational Psychology,87(3), 375–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, K., & Coplan, R. J. (2010). Exploring processes linking shyness and academic achievement in childhood. School Psychology Quarterly,25, 213–222. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B., & Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychological Science,12(1), 18–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jin, K. H. (2002). The effects of extrinsic rewards on children’s creativity (Master’s thesis). Seoul: Sungkyunkwan University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kang, H., & Park, H. (2013). The development of infants from low-income families, parenting characteristics, and daily routines. Family and Environment Research,51(6), 613–622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y., & Ahn, S. (2014). Types of motivation in young children: Associations with young children’s temperament and their mothers interactions. Korean Journal of Child Studies,35(4), 123–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K., Doh, H., & Park, S. (2010). The relationship among parenting behaviors, children’s perfectionism and achievement motivation. Korean Journal of Child Studies,31(2), 209–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., & Gwak, K. J. (2007). Validity of the Korean early childhood home observation for measurement of the environment. Korean Journal of Child Studies,28(1), 115–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., Jung, H., Kim, J., & Yi, S. (2012). Development of a Korean Home Environment Scale for early childhood. The Journal of Child Education,21(1), 77–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Fishbach, A., Woolley, K., Bélanger, J. J., Chernikova, M., Molinario, E., et al. (2018). A structural model of intrinsic motivation: On the psychology of means-ends fusion. Psychological Review,125(2), 165–182. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000095.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in the classroom: Age differences and academic correlates. Journal of Educational Psychology,97(2), 184–196. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lim, J., & Bae, Y. (2015). Validation study of Korean version of the Rothbart’s children’s behavior questionnaire. Korean Journal of Human Ecology,24(4), 477–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Schattke, K. (2018). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Time for expansion and clarification. Motivation Science. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacPhee, D., Prendergast, S., Albrecht, E., Walker, A. K., & Miller-Heyl, J. (2018). The child-rearing environment and children’s mastery motivation as contributors to school readiness. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,56, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meij, H. T., Riksen-Walraven, J. M., & Van Lieshout, C. F. (2000). Longitudinal patterns of parental support as predictors of children’s competence motivation. Early Child Development and Care,160(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/0030443001600101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, G. A., Harmon, R. J., & Maslin-Cole, C. A. (1990). Mastery motivation: Definition and measurement. Early Education and Development,1(5), 318–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muola, J. M. (2010). A study of the relationship between academic achievement motivation and home environment among standard eight pupils. Educational Research and Reviews,5(5), 213–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • OECD. (2017). Starting strong 2017—key OECD indicators on early childhood education and care. Paris: OECD Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • OECD. (2018). Education at a glance 2018: OECD indicators. Paris: OECD Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Onatsu-Arvilommi, T., & Nurmi, J. (2000). The role of task-avoidant and task-focused behaviors in the development of reading and mathematical skills during the first school year: A cross-lagged longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology,92(3), 478–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M., & Rothbart, M. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology,12(3), 427–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The children’s behavior questionnaire. Child Development,72(5), 1394–1408. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. Advances in developmental psychology (pp. 37–86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Jones, L. B. (1998). Temperament, self-regulation, and education. School Psychology Review,27(4), 479–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U., Stutz, F., & Schaffner, E. (2016). Longitudinal relations between reading motivation and reading comprehension in the early elementary grades. Learning and Individual Differences,51, 49–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, G. F. (1993). Socioeconomic advantage and achievement motivation: Important mediators of academic performance in minority children in urban schools. The Urban Review,25(3), 221–232. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01112109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Statistics Korea. (2018). Household income by income level. http://kostat.go.kr.

  • Steinmayr, R., & Spinath, B. (2009). The importance of motivation as a predictor of school achievement. Learning and Individual Differences,19(1), 80–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taber, K. S. (2018). The use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Research in Science Education,48(6), 1273–1296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Totsika, V., & Sylva, K. (2004). The home observation for measurement of the environment revisited. Child and Adolescent Mental Health,9(1), 25–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valiente, C., Swanson, J., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2012). Kindergartener’s temperament, classroom engagement, and student-teacher relationship: Moderation by effortful control. Social Development,21, 558–576.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wachs, T. D. (1987). Short-term stability of aggregated and nonaggregated measures of parent behavior. Child Development,58, 796–797. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, J. H. Y., & Guthrie, J. T. (2004). Modeling the effects of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, amount of reading, and past reading achievement on text comprehension between US and Chinese students. Reading Research Quarterly,39(2), 162–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. T. (1997). Relations of children’s motivation for reading to the amount and breadth or their reading. Journal of Educational Psychology,89(3), 420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegert, D. I., Kistner, J. A., Castro, R., & Robertson, B. (2001). Longitudinal study of young children’s responses to challenging achievement situations. Child Development,72(2), 609–624. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hye-Jung Cho.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Items of Motivation Type Questionnaire (Jin 2002)

Motivation type

Items

# of items

Cronbach’s α

Intrinsic Motivation

1. While engaged in the activities, the child feels he/she is learning what he/she really wanted to know

2. When asked to choose one activity among others, the child thinks first which activity he/she enjoys the most before making a choice

3. The child is engaged in the activities because he/she is curious and enjoys learning

4. While engaged in the activity, the child enjoys and challenges him/herself

5. While engaged in the activity, the child prefers to figure things out independently

6. While engaged in the activity, the child likes to choose his/her own activity theme (topic) and the process independently

7. The child really enjoys the activities

8. While engaged in the activity, the child enjoys it so much that he/she forgets about everything else

8

.89

Extrinsic Motivation

1. The child wants his/her teacher (parents) to know how good he/she is in the activity involved

2. The child wants to receive compliments through the activity

3. The child participates in the activity because others (i.e., teacher, parents) want him/her to

4. While engaged in the activity, the child is concerned about what other people (i.e., teacher, peer) think of about him or her

5. The child does not want to participate in the activity that is not recognized by others

6. The child prefers having the teacher set goals and provides the instructions for him/her to follow

7. The child does not like to participate in the activity if he/she has a choice not to participate

8. The child really wants to receive an award or present while engaged in the activity

8

.73

Appendix 2

Constructs of CBQ-Short version Scale (Rothbart et al. 2001)

Dimension

Constructs [item number of the original scale]

# of items used in the study

Cronbach’s α

Approach

Amount of excitement and positive anticipation for expected pleasurable activities [15, 46, 58]

3

.59

High-intensity pleasure

Amount of pleasure or enjoyment related to situations involving high stimulus intensity, rate, complexity, novelty, and incongruity [4, 10, 33, 69, 78*, 88]

6

.67

Smiling laughter

Amount of positive affect in response to changes in stimulus intensity, rate, complexity, and incongruity [19*, 48*, 79, 80*]

4

.62

Activity level

Level of gross motor activity including rate and extent of locomotion [1, 12, 18*, 22, 50*, 85, 93*]

7

.63

Impulsivity

Spread of response initiation [7, 28, 36*, 43*, 51, 82*]

6

.62

Fear

Amount of negative affect, including unease, worry or nervousness related to anticipated pain or distress and/or potentially threatening situations [17, 23, 35*, 41, 63, 68*]

6

.77

Anger/Frustration

Amount of negative affect related to the interruption of ongoing tasks or goal blocking [2, 14, 30, 40, 61*, 87]

6

.72

Inhibitory control

The capacity to plan and to suppress inappropriate approach responses under instructions or in novel or uncertain situations [38, 45, 53*, 67, 73, 81]

6

.70

Attentional Focusing

Tendency to maintain attentional focus upon task-related channels [16*, 21*, 62, 71 84*, 89]

6

.70

Low-intensity pleasure

Amount of pleasure or enjoyment related to situations involving low stimulus intensity, rate, complexity, novelty, and incongruity [26, 39, 57, 65, 72, 76, 86, 94]

8

.62

Perceptual sensitivity

Amount of detection of slight, low-intensity stimuli from the external environment

[5, 13, 24, 32, 47, 83*]

6

.71

  1. (*) indicates reverse coded
  2. The original version is available at https://research.bowdoin.edu/rothbart-temperament-questionnaires/

Appendix 3

Items of the Korean Home Environment Scale for Infants’ and Toddlers’ Homes (Kim et al. 2012)

Dimension

Items

# of Items

Cronbach’s α

Developmental stimulus

1. There are more than X CD(s) or tape(s) of children’s song at home

2. There are more than X toy(s) (dolls, materials for hospital play, dramatic play, and etc.) permitting role play

3. There are more than X artwork(s) (artworks bought for decorations or done by the child) displayed at home

4. There are more than X number-construction play material(s) (blocks, puzzles, Frobel’s gift, Gabe and etc.) at home

5. There are more than X Korean alphabet learning material(s) (word cards, Korean alphabet learning videos and etc.) at home

6. There are more than X toys permitting free expressions

7. I do Korean language learning activities(s) (reading, writing, using word cards, watching Korean alphabet learning videos and etc.) together with the child

7

.79

Responsivity

8. I try to help my child develop at his or her own level of development (I strive to provide an educational environment that suits my child’s characteristics)

9. I try to use a variety of new words to help my child develop language

10. I usually talk with my child a lot

11. I usually compliment my child a lot

12. I usually talk using easy words that match my child’s level

13. I usually compliment my child

14. I usually talk to my child with a bright and gentle voice

7

.78

Opportunities for various experiences

15. My child is taken to the movies more than X time(s) per year

16. I visit museums or exhibitions with the child more than X time(s) per year

17. My child is taken on a family trip more than X time(s) per year

18. I do physical and outside activities with the child

4

.54

Arrangement of the daily routine

19. My child has a regular day schedule (meal, bedtime, playtime, and walks, etc.)

20. I encourage my child to sleep at regular times

21. I set a time for my child’s TV viewing time (e.g. TV viewing time is limited within 30 min)

3

.66

  1. All responses for 5-Likert-scale
  2. [#1] response scale ranged from 0 to 1 to more than 8
  3. [#2–#7, #16–#19] response scale ranged from 0 to more than 4
  4. [#8–#15, #20–#22] response scale ranged from never true to always true

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Choi, N., Cho, HJ. Temperament and Home Environment Characteristics as Predictors of Young Children’s Learning Motivation. Early Childhood Educ J 48, 607–620 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-020-01019-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-020-01019-7

Keywords

Navigation