Background

Hearing loss results from impairment of the sound conduction pathway (Bagai et al. 2006). Globally, 3.6 million patients suffer from permanent hearing loss and about 1.1 billion young adults are at risk of hearing loss due to loud noise (Murphy et al. 2018). Hearing loss has a significant effect on social communications, negatively impacts the emotional state and working capacity of patients, and results in lower life quality and social status (Agrawal et al. 2008).

Accurate self-reported hearing loss is of paramount importance for early prevention and optimum utilization of medical resources. Self-reported measuring could conveniently supply an estimation of hearing status for clinicians and researchers when audiometric measurements are not available (Mccullagh et al. 2011; Kamil et al. 2015; Liljas et al. 2016). However, it is also important to ascertain the accuracy of such self-reported hearing losses, but they have, so far, only been explored in studies typically measuring sensitivity and specificity. Thus, our study investigates four different categories of factors that can potentially influence self-reported hearing loss in young adults, namely, demographic characteristics, ear problems, living behavior, and hearing health belief.

Research methods

Subject of research

Our target population was young adults aged 18–30 years living in Zhejiang Province, China. We convenience sampled Hangzhou Normal University and Hangzhou Hospital for the Prevention and Treatment of Occupational Diseases as research sites. A total of 1800 young adults were stratified sampled from Hangzhou Normal University and 792 young adults were randomly selected from the physical examination center of Hangzhou Hospital for the Prevention and Treatment of Occupational Diseases. All subjects were given a hearing-related questionnaire and underwent a pure-tone audiometric test. The sample elimination criteria were as follows: (1) currently have or have had otitis media in the past; (2) hereditary hearing loss; (3) occupational noise exposure; (4) missing data on age, gender, hearing loss family history, or other key variables; and (5) non-responsiveness or unreliable response during audiometric examinations. According to the above-mentioned criteria, 397 young adults were excluded and 2195 subjects finally enrolled in our study.

Questionnaire survey

A pilot questionnaire survey was carried out for adjustment and improvement before the self-designed questionnaire was finally adopted. Prior to the questionnaire survey, investigators received professional training to ensure data quality. The contents of the questionnaire included demographic characteristics, family history of hearing loss, tinnitus, otalgia, and aural fullness, frequency of using headphones, probability of increasing headphone volume under noisy conditions, smoking status, self-perceived hearing condition, and hearing health belief. Tinnitus was divided into three conditions: no tinnitus within the past year, occasional tinnitus (less than or equal to once a week), and frequent tinnitus (more than or equal to twice a week). Meanwhile, otalgia and aural fullness were similarly classified. Occasional and frequent morbid conditions were subsumed into a single group in the analysis below. The use of headphones was categorized into multiple groups depending on the frequency of usage: no headphone use, once or twice a day, three to five times a day, and more than five times a day. The probability categories of increasing headphone volume under noisy conditions included less than 30%, 30–50%, 51–70%, and more than 70%. Smoking status was divided into no smoking, occasional smoking (less than one cigarette a day), and frequent smoking (more than one cigarette a day for more than a year). The health belief model, abbreviated as HBM, was a psychological model that attempted to explain and predict health behaviors and included six scale dimensions, namely, perceived susceptibility, perceived severity, perceived benefits of correcting bad behavior related to hearing, perceived barriers against correcting bad behavior related to hearing, cues to action of correcting bad behavior related to hearing, and perceived self-efficacy of improving behavior related to hearing.

Self-reported hearing loss

Self-reported hearing loss was measured by asking participants the question: “How is your hearing?”. Response categories were classified into normal hearing, mild hearing loss, moderate heating loss, and severe hearing loss. We grouped the subjects with moderate and severe hearing loss together into the “moderate-severe hearing loss” group.

Audiometric measures

Devices utilized in this research were the Madsen Itera clinical diagnostic audiometer and TDH39 headphones developed by GN Otometrics of Denmark. The audiometer was calibrated and the audiometric test performed by trained technicians in a sound-proof chamber with noise levels below 30 dB. Subjects were advised to stay away from noisy conditions for more than 12 h prior to the hearing test in order to improve accuracy. Pure-tone air-conduction hearing threshold testing at frequencies between 0.125 and 8 kHz was performed on both ears. In the audiometric examination, participants who did not respond at least once were considered as non-responsive. In order to measure the reliability of the response in participants, the 1-kHz frequency was tested twice in each ear. If the results differed by more than 10 dB, it was considered to be an unreliable response.

The average hearing threshold in the ear that performed worse was determined at frequencies of 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz, and defined in accordance with the World Health Organization (WHO). Hearing loss was defined as follows: ≤ 25 dB (normal hearing), 26–40 dB (mild hearing loss), and > 40 dB (moderate-severe hearing loss).

Grouping of the self-reported hearing loss deviation

In order to reveal the accuracy of self-reported hearing loss in young adults, self-reported hearing and measured audiometric results were compared and classified into consistency, underestimate, and overestimate groups. The latter two categories constituted self-reported hearing loss deviation. For the consistency group, both the self-reported hearing condition and the actual measured audiometric result were consistent. For the underestimate group, self-reported hearing was normal, while the actual test results showed mild or moderate-severe hearing loss, or the self-reported hearing condition was described as a mild hearing loss, but the test results indicated moderate-severe hearing loss. As for the overestimate group, self-reported hearing was mild hearing loss, whereas the actual measured audiometric results revealed normal hearing, or the self-reported hearing condition was moderate-severe hearing loss, while the actual test results showed mild hearing loss or normal hearing.

Statistical approach

The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS for Windows, version 20.0, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used in our study. Sample descriptive data were summarized using percentage distributions, means, and standard deviations. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Chi-square tests were used to analyze differences between groups. The Kruskal–Wallis test was applied to examine the distribution patterns of frequency of headphone use, probability of increasing headphone volume under noisy conditions, and smoking status. Analysis of the influential factors of self-reported hearing loss deviation was done by performing multiple logistic regression based on the adjustment for confounding factors. The consistency group served as the reference group. Statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05. For the multiple logistic regression analysis, a 95% confidence interval (CI) was calculated.

Results

Comparison between self-reported hearing loss and measured hearing loss

Among the 2195 research subjects, 69.4% deemed their hearing as normal, 30.3% self-reported mild hearing loss, and 0.3% self-reported moderate-severe hearing loss (Table 1). The measured audiometric results showed that participants with normal hearing, mild, and moderate-severe hearing loss accounted for 97.0, 2.4, and 0.6%, respectively. By comparing self-reported hearing loss and measured audiometric results, 68.4% (1501) of the total subjects were found to belong to the consistency group and 29.5% (647) in the overestimate group, of whom 643 subjects had normal hearing but self-reported mild hearing loss, with the other four subjects having normal hearing but self-reporting moderate-severe hearing loss. Young adults fell into the underestimate group in 2.1% of cases (47), including 42 young adults with self-reported normal hearing and five with self-reported mild hearing loss. Among the 42 young adults who self-reported normal hearing, 36 actually had mild hearing loss and six moderate-severe hearing loss. The five adults who self-reported mild hearing loss were, in reality, suffering from moderate-severe hearing loss.

Table 1 Comparison between self-reported hearing loss and measured hearing loss

General demographic characteristics

The average ages of subjects in the consistency, underestimate, and overestimate groups were 21.4 ± 2.5, 23.0 ± 3.2, and 20.5 ± 1.3 years, respectively, which showed a statistically significant difference (Table 2). Furthermore, we found a difference in gender distribution within the three groups. There were more male subjects (62.7%) in the underestimate group, while female subjects were greater in number in the overestimate and consistency groups, with proportions of 71.1 and 56.0%, respectively. There was also a significant difference in family history of hearing loss, with 65.6% of young adults in the overestimate group having a family history of hearing loss, but only 39.0% in the underestimate group.

Table 2 General demographic characteristics

Multiple logistic regression analysis showed that age, gender, and family history influenced the accuracy of self-reported hearing loss. Age was associated with underestimating hearing loss [OR 1.210 (1.100–1.331)] and, with increasing age, we found that it was also more difficult to overestimate hearing loss [OR 0.829 (0.788–0.872)]. Being female was associated with overestimating hearing loss compared to the male group [OR 1.566 (1.282–1.912)], with a probability of 56.6%. People with a family history of hearing loss showed a 59.6% probability of overestimating their hearing loss, which was significantly higher compared to the group without a family history of hearing loss [OR 1.596 (1.317–1.933)].

Analysis of factors that influence self-reported hearing loss

Our statistical analysis of the data was adjusted for age, gender, and family history of hearing loss. Compared to those who did not use headphones, people who used them once or twice a day were less likely to underestimate hearing loss [OR 0.491 (0.254–0.948)] (Table 3). Respondents who had a probability of 50–70% of increasing the volume of headphones in a noisy environment were less likely to underestimate hearing loss compared to those who had a less than 30% probability of turning up the volume [OR 0.357 (0.148–0.860)]. Occasional smokers underestimated hearing loss more compared to non-smokers [OR 2.428 (1.140–5.170)].

Table 3 Analysis of factors influencing self-reported hearing loss

Tinnitus was a risk factor for overestimating hearing loss [OR 1.253 (1.036–1.516)] and the probability of overestimating in the aural fullness group was higher compared to people with no aural fullness [OR 1.344 (1.079–1.675)]. Compared to participants who never used headphones, those who wore headphones once or twice a day were more likely to overestimate their hearing loss [OR 2.702 (1.947–3.750)]. Interviewees who use them three to five times a day had a high probability of overestimating their hearing loss [OR 3.443 (2.360–5.023)]. The association between using headphones more than five times a day and overestimating hearing loss was even higher [OR 3.943 (2.510–6.195)]. Compared to subjects having a less than 30% probability of increasing headphone volume under noisy conditions, those who were 30–50%, 51–70%, and over 70% more likely to do so all overestimated their hearing loss with higher probabilities. Compared to non-smokers, occasional smokers were less likely to overestimate their hearing loss [OR 0.259 (0.136–0.495)] and frequent smokers were even less likely to do so [OR 0.156 (0.056–0.435)]. Moreover, those who scored higher on perceived hearing loss severity were more likely to overestimate their hearing loss [OR 1.024 (1.006–1.043)].

Discussion

Conclusions

This study showed that the consistency rate between self-reported hearing loss and the actual audiometric test was 68.4%. Smoking had a drastically positive effect on underestimating hearing loss. Age, the frequency of using headphones, and the probability of increasing the volume of headphones in a noisy environment affected underestimating hearing loss reversely. In addition, being female, having a family history of hearing loss, tinnitus, otalgia, frequent use of headphones, the probability of increasing the headphone volume in a noisy environment, and perceived severity of hearing loss showed a positive effect on overestimating hearing loss, while age and smoking had a reverse effect on overestimating hearing loss.

What is already known on this topic?

Studies do exist which use self-reported hearing condition instead of objectively measured audiometric hearing condition (Ranganathan et al. 2011; Liljas et al. 2016). A study by Nondahl et al. (1998) found that the consistency rate between self-reported and audiometric test results was 71% among elderly people. In contrast, this rate was as low as 31.9% among factory workers (Kerr et al. 2003). Kamil et al. (2015) showed that deviations in self-reported hearing loss in the elderly were associated with age, gender, race, and education. With regard to gender, males and females tended to participate in gender-specific activities, and have divergent life expectancies and exposures; thus, there were always differences in their hearing ability (Wang et al. 2018). Additionally, the gender norms or cultural notions of masculinity influence male attitudes toward health and health service use, which probably make women overestimate hearing loss (Kim et al. 2017). Previous researches have also shown associations between occupation, stress, anxiety, tinnitus, hearing aid use, and self-reported hearing loss deviation. Tinnitus and aural fullness correlate with anxiety (Moon et al. 2018; Levo et al. 2014), a symptom of general psychological pressure which makes people vulnerable to hearing problems.

What this study adds

The study findings add to the current literature on self-reported hearing loss in several ways. We first compared self-reported hearing loss with audiometric tests in young adults and evaluated the factors that influence self-reported hearing loss deviation. In addition, our study divided participants into three hearing loss groups (normal, mild, and moderate-severe) because some studies point out that, compared to the results of the dichotomy of self-reported hearing loss, grade classification is more rigorous (Ferrite et al. 2011). The hearing threshold in our study was determined by the ear that performed worse, which may have led to more accurate identification of people who underestimate hearing loss. Moreover, beside demographics and ear problems, we also investigated a range of factors on hearing, including living behavior and hearing health belief, which only a few studies have examined to date. We found that smoking, frequency of using headphones, probability of increasing headphone volume in a noisy environment, and perceived severity of hearing health belief were the most influential factors of self-reported hearing loss deviation, for example. Nicotine may have damaging effects on the auditory system (Nomura et al. 2005) and it may influence the nervous system to some extent (Durazzo et al. 2017), which might, in turn, reduce the sensitivity of hearing loss within a short time in young adult smokers. Short-time exposure to entertainment noise is likely to cause a temporary increase of the hearing threshold, also known as auditory fatigue, which leads to an overestimation of hearing loss in young adults (Cone et al. 2010). The model of hearing health belief can be applied in testing the possibility of changing behavior of subjects related to hearing health, as well as predicting the future of hearing conditions (Pronk et al. 2017). Saunders et al. (2016) pointed out that people with better hearing have higher perceived levels of sensitivity, severity, and benefits that are important for hearing loss prevention.

Limitations of this study

There are many influential factors obtained through self-reporting which could potentially contain measurement errors. Even though there are no studies at all to date that correlate patterns of living behavior with self-reported hearing loss, we only explored a small fraction of patterns of living behavior, a complex which needs to be investigated in more detail in future studies. Self-reported hearing loss belongs to subjective personal recognition and may be related to psychological conditions. However, this article did not study the relationship between psychological pressure, anxiety, and self-reported hearing loss, another topic which needs to be addressed in future studies.

Conclusion and implications of findings

We examined the difference between self-reported and objectively measured hearing loss by audiometric testing and identified factors contributing to self-reported hearing loss deviation in young Chinese adults. Understanding the influential factors of self-reported hearing loss deviation can provide effective information allowing clinicians to analyze the patient’s self-reported hearing condition. It could also provide a preliminary basis for researchers to investigate the validity of using self-reported hearing conditions to obtain the morbidity of hearing loss, which would present insightful new data for the prevention of hearing loss in young adults.