Abstract
Background
Evidence suggests that Mental Practice (MP) could be used to finesse surgical skills. However, MP is cognitively demanding and may be dependent on the ability of individuals to produce mental images. In this study, we hypothesised that the provision of interactive 3D visual aids during MP could facilitate surgical skill performance.
Methods
20 surgical trainees were case-matched to one of three different preparation methods prior to performing a simulated Laparoscopic Cholecystectomy (LC). Two intervention groups underwent a 25-minute MP session; one with interactive 3D visual aids depicting the relevant surgical anatomy (3D-MP group, n = 5) and one without (MP-Only, n = 5). A control group (n = 10) watched a didactic video of a real LC. Scores relating to technical performance and safety were recorded by a surgical simulator.
Results
The Control group took longer to complete the procedure relative to the 3D&MP condition (p = .002). The number of movements was also statistically different across groups (p = .001), with the 3D&MP group making fewer movements relative to controls (p = .001). Likewise, the control group moved further in comparison to the 3D&MP condition and the MP-Only condition (p = .004). No reliable differences were observed for safety metrics.
Conclusion
These data provide evidence for the potential value of MP in improving performance. Furthermore, they suggest that 3D interactive visual aids during MP could potentially enhance performance, beyond the benefits of MP alone. These findings pave the way for future RCTs on surgical preparation and performance.
Mental Practice (MP) can be defined as the cognitive practice of a task without overt physical movement. The approach is used routinely in sports and performing arts to enhance performance [1–5]. More recently its potential has been explored for explicit learning of surgical skills [6], but there remains a number of questions about MP’s effective implementation in the field of surgery, as studies had mixed results [7–15].
MP is a demanding process involving image generation, maintenance, inspection and transformation [16–18]. In MP, an image can be generated from direct perceptual information [19] or from resurfacing information previously stored in long-term memory [20]. A potential explanation for the lack of agreement amongst studies on the benefits of MP is that the quality of the mental image generated can vary substantially across individuals [21, 22] and is, of course, altered by one’s experiences [23, 24].
The cognitive resources required for image retention are notably high [25–27], requiring increased amounts of sustained attention [19, 28, 29] to prevent the generated image from fading in milliseconds. This is achieved mainly through the stimulation of visual memory through processing the properties of the objects found in the generated image [18]. This involves morphological assessment [20] and image manipulation [19, 29] (such as rotation, transportation [16, 30] or image reconstruction [31]). We speculated that cognitive demands may constrain the quality of the MP process and thus, impact on the benefit that could be accrued from this approach. Thus, we hypothesised that the provision of additional resources, such as interactive 3D models of the task-relevant anatomy, during MP should facilitate the process and subsequently impact on performance in trainee surgeons.
Materials and methods
Ethical approval
After consultation with the Research and Development (R&D) department of Leeds Teaching Hospitals, it was advised that approval by an NHS research ethics committee or the R&D department was not required. The study received departmental approval by the research lead of the surgical Clinical Service Unit (CSU).
Participants
Twenty junior specialty trainees (core trainees and early registrar years; postgraduate years 2–4) were recruited for this study. The surgeons were case-matched 1:1:2 (MPO: 3D&MP: Control) based on the following variables: number of real laparoscopic cholecystectomies conducted as primary surgeon and number of times they had used the same virtual reality simulator in the past (Table 1). All participants had seen and assisted in Laparoscopic Cholecystectomy (LC) operations but had not performed more than 15 as primary surgeons. Twice as many participants were allocated to the control group in order to increase statistical power [32]. This is particularly desirable if the cost for including additional control is minimal [33]. Specifically, asking individuals allocated to the control condition to review a pre-prepared didactic video bears no additional cost to the study and is a method that these surgical trainees were familiar with as it is used during teaching sessions of surgical skills within the area.
3D model
The 3D model was reconstructed from an anonymised Computed Tomography (CT) transferred through a Compact Disc (CD) in Digital Imaging and Communications in Medicine (DICOM) form, to “in-house” 3D reconstruction software (VolumeViewer, University of Leeds). The 3D model was created through manual reconstruction to “match” the VR-simulated images of a normal anatomy gallbladder, biliary tract and vascularity (Fig. 1). The model was then exported onto open source visualisation software (MeshLab) and underwent minor contouring.
Intervention
The Control Group (n = 10) was exposed to a didactic real time video of a LC, whilst two intervention groups (3D group; n = 5) and Mental Practice Only (MPO; n = 5) underwent a single 25-min Mental Preparation (MP) session in the presence of a facilitator. For the 3D group, an interactive 3D model of the relevant surgical anatomy (Fig. 1) was incorporated into the MP process (Fig. 2).
Prior to the commencement of MP, the experimenter, acting as a facilitator, provided demonstrations of verbalised mental preparation to the participants. In addition, the opportunity to train on how to use the VR simulator (LAP Mentor, Simbionix, Cleveland, OH, USA) was provided. The participants were taught practical aspects of simulation usage, such as how to select a surgical tool and where the diathermy pedals are located. The trainees did not require additional training on how to perform a laparoscopic cholecystectomy as they had previously assisted or/and performed laparoscopic cholecystectomies.
The intervention groups (MPO and 3D) were given an excerpt from a surgical textbook [34] containing a step-by-step breakdown of a LC and were asked to “visualise” and “feel” the operation. Participants allocated to the 3D group were taught how to use the rotation and zoom-in/-out tools of the visualisation software. In addition to MP, they were instructed to inspect the 3D anatomy on the virtual model for each step of the procedure.
All groups, after undergoing the appropriate preparation process, proceeded to perform a simulated laparoscopic cholecystectomy on a VR simulator. Performance (Instrumental Tip Path Length [PL], Number of Movements [NOM] and Time To Extract Gallbladder [TTGB]) and safety metrics (Number of Non-Cauterised Bleedings [NCB], number of Perforations [Per], number of Damage to Vital Structures [DVS]) automatically provided by the simulator, were recorded. We chose these performance metrics because previous research has demonstrated that they have predictive validity between experts and novices [35]. There was no previous validation of the safety metrics recorded in this study; however, these metrics were selected ahead of other measures on the basis that poor performance on these would have a clear impact on patient wellbeing if these operations were to be performed in a real clinical setting.
Statistical analysis
The performance and safety metrics were tested for departures from normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test before being subjected to a One-Way ANOVA or a non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis test as appropriate. The Shapiro–Wilk test indicated that the performance metrics were normally distributed (Time p = .87, NOM p = .67, PL p = .93). However, safety measures were demonstrated to have a non-normal distribution (p < .001). As a result of the normality testing, one-way ANOVA was used for the performance metrics and Kruskal–Wallis testing was used for the safety metrics. For the ANOVA, when a significant difference for a main effect (p < .05) was found, Bonferroni-corrected post hoc comparisons were performed. For brevity, only statistically significant post hoc comparisons are reported. We report partial eta squared values (η p ²) to indicate the effect size. An estimate of the effect size w² is reported (H/N). Analysis was carried out using IBM SPSS® version 22 (IBM, Armonk, NY) and GraphPad Prism 6.0 (GraphPad Software, Inc., California, USA).
In addition to this analysis, we also computed the Bayes factor (BF) for each of our outcome measures [36] (Table 2) using JASP Statistics (v 0.7.5.6) [37]. This approach has several advantages relative to traditional null hypothesis significance testing (p values) [38] and we report it here for two reasons. First, BFs allow us to determine whether the obtained evidence favoured the alternative hypothesis (there is a difference in outcome measures for our three conditions- with a BF > 1), the null hypothesis (BF < 1) or neither (BF equal or close to 1). This is particularly important for understanding the data when p values are greater than our significance threshold of 0.05 [39]. Second, it is particularly advantageous to use the BF when results need to be directly comparable with future work. This is because the obtained value represents a ratio of the probability of the null and alternative hypotheses and thus it is not biased by sample size [40]. To interpret the strength of the BFs, we considered a BF ranging from 1 to 3 as barely consequential evidence and a BF greater than 10 as “strong evidence” [41].
Results
Bayes factors
Examining the BF values indicated that there was strong evidence that the data obtained were 17.71, 52.42 and 12.35 (Table 2) more likely under the alternative hypothesis (that there is a difference across conditions) for Time, Total Number of Movements and Path Length, respectively, than the null hypothesis. This analysis also shows that there was more evidence for the null hypothesis than the alternative for all three safety metrics.
Performance metrics
Results from our frequentist analysis were consistent with the BF calculations. We found that performance differences across groups (Fig. 3) showed a main effect in the amount of time taken by participants to complete the simulated LC (F(2,17) = 8.77, p = .002, η p ² = 0.51), with the Control group (M = 1447, SD = 341) taking significantly longer (p = .002) relative to the 3D & MP group (M = 670, SD = 326). Similarly, the NOM was also significantly different across groups (F(2,17) = 11.57, p = .001, η p ² = 0.58), with the 3D & MP groups (M = 627.2, SD = 352) making fewer movements relative to controls (p = .001). For PL, a significant main effect was found (F(2,17) = 7.57, p = .004 η p ² = 0.47) and we observed that the control condition (M = 2837, SD = 633) led to longer distances covered in comparison to the 3D & MP condition (M = 1540, SD = 957, p = .008) and the MP-only condition (M = 1800, SD = 370; p = .038).
Safety metrics
For the safety measures (Fig. 4), no statistically significant difference was found in the frequency of the damage to vital structures (H(2) = 0.63, p = .68, ω2 = 0.03). The comparisons for non-cauterised bleeding (H(2) = 4.71, p = .13, ω2 = 0.24) and number of perforations (H(2) = 4.8, p = .082, ω2 = 0.24) showed marginal trends but did not reach the statistical significance threshold.
Discussion
There is a long history of using mental practice to improve performance in sports and arts. Whilst recent work has suggested that this approach could be adapted for skill acquisition, evidence remains equivocal [7, 8, 10]. We suggest that, because the ability to produce a mental image varies across individuals [42], this could potentially account for differences across studies. It is also known that MP is a demanding process [17, 18, 43], requiring a number of cognitive processes to work in concert [25–27]. We speculated that providing support for MP might enable trainee surgeons to maximise the benefit of this approach.
The present study therefore applied a novel approach and developed interactive 3D visual models. The hypothesis was that this would alleviate the cognitive load of producing and maintaining a virtual image and standardise the quality of the image produced amongst individuals, which we hypothesised would subsequently lead to better surgical performance. When this approach was compared with pre-procedural preparation using didactic video viewing, there was an indication from our data that this may enhance the assessed surgical performance metrics. Conversely, mental imagery alone appeared to enhance only path length when the same comparison applied.
Safety metrics (damage to vital structures, non-cauterised bleedings, liver perforations) were found to be similar in the three groups. Adverse events, with the exception of two outliers, were rare occurrences (Fig. 3), which may not have been the case if the participants recruited had been novices (i.e. have not performed the operation as a primary surgeon).
The concept of the current study is novel—no trial to date has combined a 3D model with mental practice. However, several trials have assessed the effect of mental rehearsal without the use of additional aids [7, 8, 10, 44, 45] with conflicting results. The MPO group in our study improved in only one metric (PL) when compared to the control group. Other metrics, such as NOM, showed a trend and a greater sample size study may have demonstrated a more conclusive enhancement of performance.
Similarities can also be found in the methodology described in the relevant literature. For instance, the duration of the MP sessions is similar with previous studies assessing the acquisition of surgical skills after MP [7, 8, 10, 13, 45]. Similarly to the current study, Mulla et al. [11] and Eldred-Evans et al. [45] used a step-by-step breakdown of the procedure while Sanders et al. [10] used a textbook to facilitate the consequent MP process. Other authors have applied training sessions on how to perform a laparoscopic cholecystectomy, but this was necessary because they recruited medical students who were not familiar with the performed procedure [10–14, 45]. The current study recruited advanced beginners who were familiar with the technique as surgical assistants or through performing the operation as primary surgeon.
It should be noted that there are limitations to this study. Firstly, this was an exploratory, case controlled (rather than randomised trial) with a small sample size of 20 participants. The limited sample size increases the possibility of making a type II error. Relatedly, the use of eta square calculation may overstate the effect size due to the small number of participants in the intervention groups (n = 5). As such, the generalizability of these results may be limited due to the small sample size of this study. Nevertheless, this exploratory finding does provide an interesting avenue for a future, larger scale, statistically high powered RCT. To inform future work, using an average obtained effect size from our performance metrics (η p ² = 0.52), we computed (using G*Power 3.1.9) [46] that a minimal sample size of 42 would need to be adopted to achieve 80% power (1-β error probability). We add the caveat that the eta square may be overstating the effect size in such a small sample so the sample size may need to be considerably larger. We anticipate that future studies will contribute information that will enable more accurate estimates.
It is also noteworthy that, unlike previous studies, in which medical students were recruited [10–14, 45], only surgical trainees participated—a more representative sample of the target population. Recruitment of inappropriate participants in education studies has previously been highlighted by the Association for Surgical Education (ASE) who concluded that recruiting medical students is not an appropriate method for validating educational methods that are targeted at surgical trainees [47].
To address the issue of individual differences in MP, some previous studies tested the baseline ability of the participants to perform MP in order to ensure equality of the comparative groups [12, 48]. In our study, we adopted an alternative approach (as our priority was to facilitate group level performance rather than attenuate individual differences) and instead standardised the presentation of the visual model, the presence of a facilitator and the provision of a textbook excerpt throughout the MP session.
Specialties such as orthopaedic and vascular surgery have been using imaging 3D reconstruction to make treatment decisions [49–51]. However, the anatomical model viewing process has not been employed in a systematic manner, thus providing a different experience for individual surgeons. Furthermore, three-dimensional patient-specific models have not been explored for didactic value in non-experts. Mental imagery provides an ideal platform for both a systematic approach and for boosting the potential didactic effect of anatomically variant models. This combined approach of mental imagery and anatomically variant anatomical models may be applied both in the pre-operative preparation of expert surgeons prior to complex surgical procedures or in didactic sessions for novices. For instance, minimally invasive total mesorectal excision complexity has been shown to be associated with patient (e.g. pelvic dimensions) and tumour characteristics (e.g. rectal cancer local invasion and distance from the anal verge) [52–54]; hence requiring a preparation which accommodates these factors. The merger of anatomical models reconstructed from medical images and mental preparation introduces the possibility of patient-specific rehearsals for the above as well as other types of surgery.
Varying anatomy often complicates laparoscopic cholecystectomy (the procedure used in this study). Previous anatomical or radiological studies have categorised the relevant anatomical variations [55], which we can recreate in 3D anatomical models—models that can subsequently be used in combination with mental imagery to teach non-expert trainee surgeons. This is a relatively inexpensive method (3D reconstruction software is available as freeware and requires no specialist IT experience [56]), which could potentially boost surgical performance and ultimately lead to improved patient outcomes.
References
Moran A, Guillot A, Macintyre T, Collet C (2012) Re-imagining motor imagery: building bridges between cognitive neuroscience and sport psychology. Br J Psychol 103:224–247
Morton PA (2003) The hypnotic belay in alpine mountaineering: the use of self-hypnosis for the resolution of sports injuries and for performance enhancement. Am J Clin Hypn 46:45–51
Pugh CM (2012) Warm-ups, mental rehearsals and deliberate practice: adopting the strategies of elite professionals. J Surg Res 176:404–405
Roure R, Collet C, Deschaumes-Molinaro C, Delhomme G, Dittmar A, Vernet-Maury E (1999) Imagery quality estimated by autonomic response is correlated to sporting performance enhancement. Physiol Behav 66:63–72
Lejeune M, Decker C, Sanchez X (1994) Mental rehearsal in table tennis performance. Percept Mot Skills 79:627–641
Hall JC (2002) Imagery practice and the development of surgical skills. Am J Surg 184:465–470
Arora S, Aggarwal R, Moran A, Sirimanna P, Crochet P, Darzi A, Kneebone R, Sevdalis N (2011) Mental practice: effective stress management training for novice surgeons. J Am Coll Surg 212:225–233
Arora S, Aggarwal R, Sirimanna P, Moran A, Grantcharov T, Kneebone R, Sevdalis N, Darzi A (2011) Mental practice enhances surgical technical skills: a randomized controlled study. Ann Surg 253:265–270
Louridas M, Bonrath EM, Sinclair DA, Dedy NJ, Grantcharov TP (2015) Randomized clinical trial to evaluate mental practice in enhancing advanced laparoscopic surgical performance. Br J Surg 102:37–44
Sanders CW, Sadoski M, van Walsum K, Bramson R, Wiprud R, Fossum TW (2008) Learning basic surgical skills with mental imagery: using the simulation centre in the mind. Med Educ 42:607–612
Mulla M, Sharma D, Moghul M, Kailani O, Dockery J, Ayis S, Grange P (2012) Learning basic laparoscopic skills: a randomized controlled study comparing box trainer, virtual reality simulator, and mental training. J Surg Educ 69:190–195
Jungmann F, Gockel I, Hecht H, Kuhr K, Rasanen J, Sihvo E, Lang H (2011) Impact of perceptual ability and mental imagery training on simulated laparoscopic knot-tying in surgical novices using a Nissen fundoplication model. Scand J Surg 100:78–85
Sanders CW, Sadoski M, Bramson R, Wiprud R, Van Walsum K (2004) Comparing the effects of physical practice and mental imagery rehearsal on learning basic surgical skills by medical students. Am J Obstet Gynecol 191:1811–1814
Bathalon S, Dorion D, Darveau S, Martin M (2005) Cognitive skills analysis, kinesiology, and mental imagery in the acquisition of surgical skills. J Otolaryngol 34:328–332
Rao A, Tait I, Alijani A (2015) Systematic review and meta-analysis of the role of mental training in the acquisition of technical skills in surgery. Am J Surg 210:545–553
Kosslyn SM (1980) Image and mind. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Ganis G (2006) The case for mental imagery, Oxford University Press, Inc., New York
Kosslyn SM (1994) Image and brain. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
Pearson DG (2007) Mental imagery and creative thought. Proc Br Acad 147:187–212
Pearson DG, Deeprose C, Wallace-Hadrill SM, Burnett Heyes S, Holmes EA (2013) Assessing mental imagery in clinical psychology: a review of imagery measures and a guiding framework. Clin Psychol Rev 33:1–23
Hitch GJ, Brandimonte MA, Walker P (1995) Two types of representation in vi- sual memory—evidence from the effects of stimulus contrast on image combina- tion. Mem Cogn 23:147–154
Pearson DG, Logie RH (2004) Effects of stimulus modality and working memory load on mental synthesis performance. Imagin Cogn Personal 23:183–192
Cornoldi C, Rossana DB (1998) Memory and imagery: A visual trace is not a men- tal image. In: Martin EGS AC, Cesare C (eds) Theories of Memory. Psychology Press, Hove, pp 87–110
Gardini S, Cornoldi C, De Beni R, Venneri A (2009) Cognitive and neuronal processes involved in sequential generation of general and specific mental images. Psychol Res 73:633–643
Decety J, Jeannerod M, Prablanc C (1989) The timing of mentally represented actions. Behav Brain Res 34:35–42
Decety J, Jeannerod M (1995) Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitts’s law hold in motor imagery? Behav Brain Res 72:127–134
Decety J, Grezes J (1999) Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions. Trends Cogn Sci 3:172–178
Schuster C, Hilfiker R, Amft O, Scheidhauer A, Andrews B, Butler J, Kischka U, Ettlin T (2011) Best practice for motor imagery: a systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines. BMC Med 9:75
Pearson DG, De Beni R, Cornoldi C (2001) The generation and transformation of visuo-spatial mental images. In: Denis RHLM, Cornoldi C, de Vega M, Engelkamp J (eds) Imagery, language and visuo-spatial thinking. Psychology press, Hove, pp 1–23
Shepard RN, Cooper LA (1982) Mental images and their transformations, Bradford Books, Cambridge
Finke RA, Pinker S, Farah MJ (1989) Reinterpreting visual patterns in mental imagery. Cogn Sci 13:51–78
Hennessy S, Bilker WB, Berlin JA, Strom BL (1999) Factors influencing the optimal control-to-case ratio in matched case-control studies. Am J Epidemiol 149(2):195–197
Rothman KJ (1986) Modern epidemiology, Little. Brown and Company, Boston
Skandalakis LJ Skandalakis JE, Skandalakis PN (2009) Extrahepatic biliary tract. Surgical anatomy and technique. A pocket manual, Springer, New York
Aggarwal R, Crochet P, Dias A, Misra A, Ziprin P, Darzi A (2009) Development of a virtual reality training curriculum for laparoscopic cholecystectomy. Br J Surg 96:1086–1093
Jeffreys H (1961) Theory of probability. Oxford University Press., Oxford
Love J, Selker, R., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Dropmann, D., Verhagen, A. J., Ly, A., Gronau, Q. F., Smira, M., Epskamp, S., Matzke, D., Wild, A., Knight, P., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2015) JASP (Version 0.7.5)[Computer software].
Masson ME (2011) A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing. Behav Res Methods Sep 43(3):679–690
Dienes Z (2014) Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Front Psychol 5:781
Jarosz AF, Wiley J (2014) What are the odds? A practical guide to computing and reporting Bayes factors. J Probl Solving 7:2–9
Kass RER, Adrian E (1995) Bayes Factors. J Am Stat Assoc 90:773–795
Gregg M, Hall C, Butler A (2010) The MIQ-RS: a suitable option for examining movement imagery ability. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med 7(2):249–257
Kosslyn SM (1980) Image and mind. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Immenroth M, Burger T, Brenner J, Nagelschmidt M, Eberspacher H, Troidl H (2007) Mental training in surgical education: a randomized controlled trial. Ann Surg 245:385–391
Eldred-Evans D, Grange P, Cheang A, Yamamoto H, Ayis S, Mulla M, Immenroth M, Sharma D, Reedy G (2013) Using the mind as a simulator: a randomized controlled trial of mental training. J Surg Educ 70:544–551
Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG (2009) Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods 41:1149–1160
Stefanidis D, Sevdalis N, Paige J, Zevin B, Aggarwal R, Grantcharov T, Jones DB, Association for Surgical Education Simulation C (2015) Simulation in surgery: what’s needed next? Ann Surg 261:846–853
Louridas M, Bonrath EM, Sinclair DA, Dedy NJ, Grantcharov TP (2015) Randomized clinical trial to evaluate mental practicein enhancing advanced laparoscopic surgical performance. BJS 102:37–44
Desender L, Rancic Z, Aggarwal R, Duchateau J, Glenck M, Lachat M, Vermassen F, Van Herzeele I, Everest (2013) Patient-specific rehearsal prior to EVAR: a pilot study. Eur J Vasc Endovasc Surg 45:639–647
Furnstahl P, Vlachopoulos L, Schweizer A, Fucentese SF, Koch PP (2015) Complex osteotomies of tibial plateau malunions using computer-assisted planning and patient-specific surgical guides. J Orthop Trauma 29:e270–e276
Hislop SJ, Hedrick JH, Singh MJ, Rhodes JM, Gillespie DL, Johansson M, Illig KA (2009) Simulation case rehearsals for carotid artery stenting. Eur J Vasc Endovasc Surg 38:750–754
Bertani E, Chiappa A, Della Vigna P, Radice D, Papis D, Cossu L, Biffi R, Bianchi PP, Luca F, Andreoni B (2014) The impact of pelvimetry on anastomotic leakage in a consecutive series of open, laparoscopic and robotic low anterior resections with total mesorectal excision for rectal cancer. Hepato-gastroenterology 61:1574–1581
Ananin SF, Targarona EM, Martinez C, Pernas JC, Hernandez D, Gich I, Sancho FJ, Trias M (2014) Predicting the pathological features of the mesorectum before the laparoscopic approach to rectal cancer. Surg Endosc 28:3458–3466
Targarona EM, Balague C, Pernas JC, Martinez C, Berindoague R, Gich I, Trias M (2008) Can we predict immediate outcome after laparoscopic rectal surgery? multivariate analysis of clinical, anatomic, and pathologic features after 3-dimensional reconstruction of the pelvic anatomy. Ann Surg 247:642–649
Ding YM, Wang B, Wang WX, Wang P, Yan JS (2007) New classification of the anatomic variations of cystic artery during laparoscopic cholecystectomy. World J Gastroenterol 13:5629–5634
Yushkevich PA, Piven J, Hazlett HC, Smith RG, Ho S, Gee JC, Gerig G (2006) User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of anatomical structures: significantly improved efficiency and reliability. Neuroimage 31:1116–1128
Funding
The authors report no external funding for this study. Marina Yiasemidou and Daniel Glassman were recipients of the Health Education Yorkshire and the Humber fellowship and Marina Yiasemidou received the A.G Leventis Foundation Scholarship at the time of the study.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Disclosures
Marina Yiasemidou, Daniel Glassman, Faisal Mushtaq, Christos Athanasiou, Mark-Mon Williams, David Jayne, and Danilo Miskovic declare that they have no conflict of interest or financial ties to disclose.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
About this article
Cite this article
Yiasemidou, M., Glassman, D., Mushtaq, F. et al. Mental practice with interactive 3D visual aids enhances surgical performance. Surg Endosc 31, 4111–4117 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00464-017-5459-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00464-017-5459-3
Keywords
- Mental practice
- Surgical training
- Anatomical models