Abstract
The auditory midbrain (inferior colliculus, IC) plays an important role in sound processing, acting as hub for acoustic information extraction and for the implementation of fast audio-motor behaviors. IC neurons are topographically organized according to their sound frequency preference: dorsal IC regions encode low frequencies while ventral areas respond best to high frequencies, a type of sensory map defined as tonotopy. Tonotopic maps have been studied extensively using artificial stimuli (pure tones) but our knowledge of how these maps represent information about sequences of natural, spectro-temporally rich sounds is sparse. We studied this question by conducting simultaneous extracellular recordings across IC depths in awake bats (Carollia perspicillata) that listened to sequences of natural communication and echolocation sounds. The hypothesis was that information about these two types of sound streams is represented at different IC depths since they exhibit large differences in spectral composition, i.e., echolocation covers the high-frequency portion of the bat soundscape (> 45 kHz), while communication sounds are broadband and carry most power at low frequencies (20–25 kHz). Our results showed that mutual information between neuronal responses and acoustic stimuli, as well as response redundancy in pairs of neurons recorded simultaneously, increase exponentially with IC depth. The latter occurs regardless of the sound type presented to the bats (echolocation or communication). Taken together, our results indicate the existence of mutual information and redundancy maps at the midbrain level whose response cannot be predicted based on the frequency composition of natural sounds and classic neuronal tuning curves.
Introduction
Animals depend greatly on acoustic signals to interact with the environment and other life beings. Encoding acoustic information in the auditory system is a fundamental step leading to the production of behavioral responses in everyday scenarios (Ryan et al. 1985; Brudzynski 2013; Jiang et al. 2017; Liévin-Bazin et al. 2018). The latter could determine the animals’ well-being and their capacity to adapt to environmental pressures.
The main aim of this article is to study the representation of natural sounds in the auditory midbrain (inferior colliculus, IC). The IC is an important integration hub in the auditory pathway that has been linked to the production of fast audio-motor behaviors instrumental for animal survival (Covey et al. 1987; Casseday and Covey 1996; Malmierca 2004). This structure is also a target area for auditory prostheses that benefit deaf patients who cannot sufficiently profit from cochlear implants (Colletti et al. 2007; Lim et al., 2007, 2008). Albeit the IC has been studied extensively at the anatomical and functional levels (Casseday et al. 2002; Malmierca 2004; Simmons et al. 2020), our knowledge of how neurons within this structure represent natural sound streams is still sparse.
We relied on bats as experimental animal model to assess how natural sound sequences are represented simultaneously across IC depths. Bats represent an excellent animal model for auditory experiments because of their rich soundscape, which includes echolocation (sound-based navigation) and multiple types of communication sounds (Wilkinson and Boughma 1998; Schnitzler et al. 2003). The latter are used to maintain hierarchies in the colonies, to communicate with infants and to alert other individuals about potentially dangerous/uncomfortable situations (Balcombe and McCracken 1992; Gadziola et al. 2012; Knörnschild et al. 2013).
The auditory system of bats has been heavily studied in the last decades but, at present, no consensus exists as to whether communication and echolocation sounds can be represented by the same neurons (Kössl et al. 2015). In the bat species Carollia perspicillata (the species of choice for this study), there is a clear dissociation in the frequency domain between communication and biosonar sounds used for orientation. The former cover the low-frequency portion of the bat soundscape, with the power of individual syllables peaking at frequencies close to 20 kHz, while the latter carry most energy in the high-frequency band between 45 and 100 kHz (Fig. 1; Brinkløv et al. 2011; Hechavarría et al. 2016a, b).
The bat IC follows the general mammalian plan with a dorsolateral–ventromedial tonotopic arrangement in which neurons located close to the brain surface process low frequencies, and neurons located in ventral IC layers process high frequencies (Grinnell 1963; Friauf 1992; Jen and Chen 1998; Malmierca et al. 2008). There is one peculiarity in C. perspicillata’s IC: although ventral neurons are responsive to high frequencies they respond as well to low-frequency sounds (Beetz et al. 2017). In other words, neurons located in the ventral IC of this bat species are likely to display multi-peaked frequency-tuning curves and all neurons throughout the IC’s tonotopy respond (at least to some extent) to sounds whose carrier frequency lies in the 20–30 kHz range. It has been speculated that multi-peaked frequency tuning could allow neurons to respond to both, echolocation and communication sounds (Kanwal and Rauschecker 2007; Kössl et al. 2015). We reasoned that distress utterances produced by C. perspicillata could drive activity throughout the entire IC, since both dorsal and ventral neurons are responsive to frequencies ~ 20 kHz, corresponding to the peak frequencies of distress vocalizations (Hechavarría et al. 2016a).
Using laminar probes, we performed simultaneous recordings from dorsal and ventral IC areas in awake C. perspicillata. Our hypothesis was that, in response to echolocation sequences, the information provided by collicular neurons should be highest in ventral IC regions since echolocation sounds are mostly high frequency. On the other hand, information about communication sequences could be either highest in dorsal IC or equally distributed throughout the entire structure, due to the presence of multi-peaked frequency-tuning curves in ventral areas. The data revealed that ventral IC regions are more informative than dorsal regions not only about echolocation sounds, but also about communication, which was surprising given our original hypothesis. The ventral IC also contains the highest degree of response redundancy in pairs of neurons recorded simultaneously. This redundancy is tightly linked to signal correlations in the neurons recorded. Overall, the data presented in this article provide evidence for topographical representations of acoustic information and redundancy in the mammalian midbrain in naturalistic contexts.
Results
The activity of 864 units (1 per penetration for each of the 16 recording sites on the silicon probe) was recorded from the central nucleus of the inferior colliculus (IC) in awake bats (species C. perspicillata) in response to pure tones and to 4 natural sound sequences (Fig. 1, for sequence parameters see Table 1). Note that since we did not perform anatomy in every animal, we cannot rule out the possibility that some units recorded do not belong to the central nucleus of the IC. Natural sequences consisted of three distress calls (Seq1–3) and one echolocation sequence. Distress calls are a type of communication sound used to advertise danger/discomfort to others (Russ et al. 1998, 2004; Eckenweber and Knörnschild 2016; Hechavarría et al. 2016a). The three distress sequences were chosen because they constitute typical examples of bats’ alarm utterances (Hechavarría et al. 2016a). Only one biosonar sequence was used, as echolocation is a stereotyped behavior that involves fixed action patterns as bats approach a target (Neuweiler 1990, 2003; Thies et al. 1998). The echolocation sequence was recorded in a pendulum paradigm in which a bat was swung towards a reflective wall (Beetz et al. 2016a). Distress and echolocation sequences have been used in previous studies characterizing the bat auditory system (Beetz et al. 2016a, 2017; Hechavarría et al. 2016b; Wohlgemuth and Moss 2016; Martin et al. 2017; García-Rosales et al. 2018; Macías et al. 2018). To restrict the study to units that responded to the calls, we considered only those units that carried at least 1 bit/s of information (Kayser et al. 2009) in response to at least one of the sequences studied (864 units out of 976). In addition, frequency-tuning curves were considered only for units that responded reliably to pure tones, i.e., they fired at least 6 spikes to all frequencies tested (814/864 units studied, ~ 94%). Manual inspection of the tuning curves was performed to double check that this arbitrary criterion provided consistent results.
General properties of C. perspicillata’s auditory midbrain
Iso-level frequency-tuning curves (FTCs) were analyzed to confirm the tonotopy along the dorsolateral–ventromedial axis of the IC’s central nucleus (Grinnell 1963; Friauf 1992; Jen and Chen 1998; Malmierca et al. 2008; Beetz et al. 2017). The inferior colliculus is functionally organized in iso-frequency layers, with each layer being sensitive to a narrow range of frequencies, from low to high frequencies in the dorsolateral–ventromedial axis (Friauf 1992; Malmierca et al. 2008), which we approximate with dorso–ventral recordings. We analyzed the response to pure tones (frequencies from 10 to 90 kHz, steps of 5 kHz, 60-dB SPL) in terms of number of spikes to create iso-level FTCs. Figure 2a shows the results of an example recording and the iso-level FTCs obtained in all 16 channels simultaneously. Two peaks of high activity are evident, especially in deep IC areas. The high-frequency peak shifts to higher frequencies as the depth of the channels increases which demonstrates the tonotopy of the inferior colliculus (Fig. 2b, population data, 814 units, see exclusion criteria for frequency-tuning curves above). The low-frequency peak (10–30 kHz; Fig. 2a) occurs throughout all depths studied. Note that multi-peaked FTCs have been reported before in the bats’ IC (Casseday and Covey 1992; Mittmann and Wenstrup 1995; Holmstrom et al. 2007; Beetz et al. 2017), as well as in the AC of bats and other species (Sutter and Schreiner 1991; Fitzpatrick et al. 1993; Hagemann et al. 2011) and frontal auditory areas (López-Jury et al. 2019).
Tonotopy in the inferior colliculus. a Normalized (for each channel) number of spikes of the recording #11 plotted against the channel depth (relative to the most dorsal channel). b Scatter plot of the depth (in relation to the brain’s surface) and iso-level best frequency (iBF) of each unit. Note the general increase in iBF as the depth increases. In red, correlation coefficient (R) of the exponential curve fitted into the data with the bisquare robust method and equation: f(x) = 7.9e0.0008x (x: depth in µm, f(x): iBF in kHz). In the background are violin plots of the iBF per discretized depth (each violin considers units within a 0.5 depth range; note that the violin width is narrower than the considered depth range). The circle represents the median and the gray line represents the interquartile range. Only considered units that responded with > 6 spikes to all frequencies tested in the frequency-tuning protocol (n = 814). c Proportion of multi-peaked iso-level FTCs. Multi-peaked units had peaks in the frequency ranges of 10–35 kHz and 40–90 kHz (peak defined as > 85% of the maximum spike-count value). Note the increase of multi-peaked units in more ventral regions. d Histogram of the iBFs (n = 814). e Spectra of the natural calls used in this study with normalized SPL
As expected from the known tonotopy of the IC, there was a positive correlation between neuronal best frequency (i.e., frequency that triggers the largest number of spikes; from now on, iso-level best frequency, iBF) and the depth of the channel that recorded the units (Fig. 2b). In addition, as IC depth increased, so did the probability of finding units with complex FTCs having more than one peak (multi-peaked FTCs, Fig. 2a, c). Note, however, that the two-peak population could be underrepresented, as above threshold levels could lead to inhibition at the characteristic frequency (Gaucher et al. 2020). At the population level, there was an overrepresentation of low iBF (10–30 kHz), likely influenced by the fact that neurons tuned to high frequencies were also responsive to low-frequency tones (Fig. 2d). Note that the range from 10 to 30 kHz corresponds to the peak frequencies in distress calls (Figs. 1 and 2e).
Based on the results obtained with pure tones, one could speculate that distress sounds (with peak frequencies at 20–30 kHz) should be best represented in dorsal IC layers or throughout the extent of the IC. On the other hand, echolocation sounds should drive strongest spiking in deep IC regions responsive to high frequencies.
Ventral units in the inferior colliculus are better trackers of natural auditory streams
The main aim of this study was to assess whether there is a difference in information representation in response to natural sounds across IC depths. As stimuli, we used natural distress sequences, which carry most power in low frequencies (20–30 kHz), and an echolocation sequence with high power in frequencies ranging from 60 to 90 kHz (see stimuli in Fig. 1 and stimuli spectra in Fig. 2e).
A qualitative check of the neural responses to the sequences already revealed that dorsal units were worse in representing natural sound streams than ventral units, regardless of the type of stimulus presented (distress or echolocation). Ventral units appear more precise and reliable across trials in their responses to both distress and echolocation sequences (see example dorsal and ventral units in Fig. 3c, f and Fig. 3d, g, respectively).
Ventral units represent more accurately the stimuli. a Frequency tuning curves of one exemplary dorsal unit (top, in red) and ventral unit (bottom, in blue). While the dorsal unit has a clear peak in the low frequencies, the ventral unit shows a double-peaked curve, one low- and the other high-frequency peak. b, e Oscillograms of the natural calls used as stimuli. c, d, f, g Raster plots (50 trials in total; top) and peristimulus time histogram (PSTH; 1-ms precision; bottom) of one dorsal unit (c, f; same as in a top) and one ventral unit (d, g; same as in a bottom) in response to the sequences shown in b and e. Note the higher precision and reliability in the ventral unit
Differences between dorsal and ventral units regarding the information they provide about natural sound streams were quantified by means of Shannon’s mutual information. Mutual information calculations capture all nonlinear dependencies between the response and the stimulus, and do not make assumptions about which stimulus features trigger the responses. The information based on a code defined by the spiking rate (Irate; see Methods) provided by the units increased exponentially with IC depth, regardless of the type of sequence (i.e., distress or echolocation) used as stimulus (Fig. 4). In other words, observing the neuronal response of ventral units reduces more the uncertainty about the stimulus than the response from dorsal units, and this trend was independent of the sounds heard. As shown in Fig. 4a, Irate and IC depth had an exponential relation (the exponential fit had a higher adjusted r-squared value than the linear fit, adjusted R2linear = 0.22 vs adjusted R2exp = 0.25), with increasing Irate with IC depth. Note that some units have negative information values due to the stringent effect of the bias correction on the information estimates (see Methods). To statistically compare the Irate at different depths, we classified the Irate estimates into five depth groups, and corroborated that the ventral units carry more information than dorsal units (Fig. 4b; FDR-corrected Wilcoxon rank-sum tests, p < 0.05) across all sound sequences tested. The same analyses with the additional segregation of single and multi-peaked units did not show substantial differences between these two groups (Fig. S2). Furthermore, we recalculated the information values considering the latency for each unit, i.e., shifting the response window so that it starts at the unit’s latency and found similar results to the ones obtained without latency correction (Fig. S3).
Ventral units carry the most information regardless of the stimuli. a Scatter plots of the information in the rate code plotted against the depth of the recorded unit for all sequences. In red is the exponential curve fitted to the data with the corresponding correlation coefficient value. b Violin plots of the information as in a with discretized depths for all sequences (median in circle, interquartile range in gray line). Each violin represents the information of the units comprised in 0.5-mm depth distance with center at the value stated in the labels. Statistical comparisons were performed by the FDR-corrected Wilcoxon rank-sum tests. The insets show p value matrices of all the statistical comparisons in a logarithmic scale. * pcorr < 0.05
Finding an exponential relation between IC depth and mutual information was an unexpected result considering the tonotopic characteristics of the IC and the disparate spectral structure of the stimuli tested (echolocation vs. distress). Our results suggest the presence of a topographical representation of mutual information throughout the IC, presumably linked to the large complexity of receptive fields in ventral IC units and the complex spectra of natural sounds (see “Discussion”).
Joint information in groups of neurons enables better representation of acoustic stimuli
The information provided by units recorded simultaneously was also quantified by means of joint information calculations (Ijoint, calculated in a total of 5237 pairs). Ijoint measures the information considering responses in pairs of units as their combined activity, considering the identity of individual responses (i.e., which unit fired which spikes; see Methods). Note that a unit can be considered multiple times to form pairs with other units, since we recorded simultaneously from 16 IC loci.
The maximum Irate estimates of the individual units that composed each pair (Irate_max) were compared to their Ijoint (Fig. 5a). This comparison tests whether more information is provided by responses of pairs of units than by a unit separately. The results showed that Ijoint was significantly higher than Irate_max regardless of the stimulation sequence considered (FDR-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, p < 0.05). Thus, in the IC, the response of two simultaneously recorded units provides more information than the response of a single unit for the tested stimuli. Besides pairs, the information of the spike rate was also calculated for larger groups of units (Fig. S4): triplets (n = 16,703), quadruplets (n = 31,112) and quintuplets (n = 33,042). As expected, the information increased with the number of units considered (FDR-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, p < 0.05), i.e., with higher number of units used to calculate the mutual information, the more uncertainty of the stimulus was reduced.
Redundancy increases with depth in simultaneously recorded units. a Quantitative analyses of the maximum Irate of the units that form the pairs (Irate_max) and Ijoint per sequence. Blue lines depict pairs which had Irate_max < Ijoint and red lines pairs which had Irate_max > Ijoint. Percentage of pairs in each situation is shown on top. Statistical comparisons performed by FDR-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. ***pcorr < 0.001. b Quantitative analyses of the sum of information of the rate codes of each of the units that form the pairs (Irate_sum) and the Ijoint per sequence. Blue lines depict pairs which had Irate_sum > Ijoint and red lines pairs which had Irate_sum < Ijoint. Percentage of pairs in each situation is shown on top. Statistical comparisons performed by FDR-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. ***pcorr < 0.001. c Scatter plots of the Ii estimates (Irate_sum – Ijoint; Irate_sum = Irate(a) + Irate(b)) plotted against the intermediate depth of the pairs (only those pairs with units distanced by 100 µm were considered). In red is the exponential curve fitted to the data with the corresponding correlation coefficient value. d Violin plots of the Ii as in C with discretized depths for all sequences. Each violin represents the Ii of the pairs comprised in 0.5-mm depth distance with the center at the value stated in the labels. Statistical comparisons were performed by the FDR-corrected Wilcoxon rank-sum tests. The insets show p value matrices of all the statistical comparisons in a logarithmic scale. *pcorr < 0.05
An information redundancy map exists in the auditory midbrain
To estimate if pairs of units carried redundant information, Ijoint was statistically compared to the linear sum of Irate of the units that formed the pairs (Irate_sum). Irate_sum was significantly higher than Ijoint in all the sequences considered (Fig. 5b; FDR-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, pcorr < 0.001), although to a lesser extent in the echolocation sequence, i.e., Irate_sum was larger than Ijoint only in 56% of the cases studied for echolocation vs. 70% for the three distress sequences. Overall, our results indicate that the population of simultaneously recorded units share information, at least to some degree. Thus, in response to natural sound streams, the auditory midbrain displays some degree of redundant information representation.
The degree of redundancy in unit pairs was quantified by computing the information interaction (Ii) as the difference between the Irate_sum and the Ijoint for each neuronal pair. Ii calculations can yield three possible outcomes: (1) redundant representations (Ii > 0, indicating shared information between units); (2) synergy (Ii < 0, both units provide bonus information when studied simultaneously); and (3) independence (Ii = 0, units provide the same information when considered together and the sum when considered separately). Plotting Ii values vs. midbrain depth (mean depth of the two units forming each pair) revealed an exponential relation between these two variables, irrespectively of the stimulus presented to the bat (Fig. 5c). In other words, the highest Ii values (indicating more redundancy) are found in pairs of neurons recorded in deep-midbrain layers. This trend was statistically validated by comparing redundancy across depth groups (Fig. 5d; FDR-corrected Wilcoxon rank-sum tests, p < 0.05), which showed a significant increase of Ii from ~ 2-mm depth. Taken together, our results suggest that the ventral IC provides more informative, but also more redundant representations of natural incoming communication and echolocation sound streams.
Redundancy is highest in nearby units and arises from signal correlations in unit pairs
To unveil the origins of the redundant representations observed, we separated unit pairs according to whether they showed redundant or synergistic interactions (Ii > 0 and Ii < 0, respectively). The Ii values were then analyzed considering the anatomical distance between the units forming the pairs (Fig. 6a–d). For this analysis, data from all stimulation sequences were pooled together. When considering only the redundant pairs, nearby units had higher redundancy levels than distant units (Fig. 6a). Even though the number of pairs decreased with inter-unit distance (see inset histogram), statistical comparisons between nearby and faraway units were significant (Fig. 6b). In the case of synergistic pairs (Fig. 6c, d), we did not observe a clear dependence of Ii with depth although there was a small increase in Ii values for pairs in which the units were located far away from each other. In other words, it appeared as if information redundancy was more likely to occur when units were close to each other, while synergy tended to be higher in pairs of distant units.
The redundancy is higher in nearby units and mostly comes from signal correlations. a Violin plot of the redundancy values (median in circle, interquartile range in gray line) for those redundant pairs (i.e., that had Ii > 0) displayed according to the distance between the units that form the pairs. Light boxes display a zoom-in of the median and interquartile range of the nearby violin plot (axis on the right). The inset shows the histogram of the pairs used for each distance. b p value matrix (FDR-corrected Wilcoxon rank-sum tests) with a logarithmic scale. *pcorr < 0.05 for statistical comparisons from (a). c and d with the same specifications for a and b, respectively, but for synergistic pairs (Ii < 0). e Scatter plot of the Ii estimates shown against the correlation coefficients between the frequency tuning of the units forming the pairs (n = 5237). In red is the exponential curve fitted to the data with the corresponding correlation coefficient value. f Quantitative analysis of the Ii broken down into signal (Isign) and noise (Inoise) correlations for each pair and stimulus. Blue lines depict pairs which had Isign > Inoise and red lines pairs which had Isign < Inoise. Percentage of pairs in each situation is shown on top. Statistical comparisons performed by FDR-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. FTC: frequency-tuning curve. *** pcorr < 0.001
We also tested whether redundancy depended on units having similar iso-level frequency-tuning curves. To that end, Ii was analyzed considering the Pearson correlation coefficients between the FTCs of units forming the pairs (Fig. 6e). There was a moderate dependence between these two variables resulting in a correlation coefficient of 0.23 using an exponential fit (Fig. 6e). This shows a weak tendency towards higher correlated FTCs having more redundancy.
In a last step, we separated Ii into two components: (1) the contribution from signal correlations (Isign) and (2) from noise correlations (Inoise). Isign captures the similarity between the average response in the two neurons studied across different time frames of the same stimulation sequence, i.e., the degree to which the average response changes with the substimulus. Isign is expected to be high in neurons with similar tuning properties (Averbeck et al. 2006). On the other hand, Inoise refers to the trial-by-trial variability in the responses (Averbeck et al. 2006). Noise correlations do not consider the impact of shared stimulation as they are quantified for “fixed” substimuli (Magri et al. 2009). While Isign always results in redundancy, Inoise can lead to either redundancy or synergy. Our data show that in the IC, most of the redundancy between two units results from signal correlations (Fig. 6f). Thus, we can conclude that the redundancy observed in the IC is mostly stimulus-driven and does not necessarily represent an internal feature (noise) of the neuronal pairs.
Discussion
In this study, we conducted simultaneous recordings of neuronal activity across the entire dorso–ventral extent of the inferior colliculus in awake bats presented with natural sound sequences. Our analysis focused on the spatial pattern of information representation at the midbrain level in response to natural sound streams. This is an important aspect for identifying which parts of the IC are instrumental for conveying information to other brain structures, such as the auditory thalamus, cortex and sensory-motor structures. Moreover, understanding how natural utterances are represented in the IC has direct translational implications, as this structure is a target area for prostheses aimed to help patients who cannot benefit from cochlear implants (Lim et al. 2009).
Our main findings are: (1) in bats, neurons carrying the most information about both distress and echolocation sequences are located ventrally in the IC; (2) unit pairs in ventral regions carry the highest redundancy as well; and (3) redundancy arises mostly from signal correlations in the units’ responses and is highest in nearby units with similar receptive fields.
Ventral IC units have complex receptive fields and are highly informative about natural sound streams
In agreement with previous studies, we observed that the bat auditory midbrain contains units with multi-peaked FTCs (Casseday and Covey 1992; Mittmann and Wenstrup 1995; Holmstrom et al. 2007; Beetz et al. 2017). In C. perspicillata, these units fire strongly to both low-frequency (10–30 kHz) and high-frequency sounds with a response notch in between (see example tuning curves in Figs. 2a and 3a) and they are more likely to be found in ventral IC areas, as seen also in a study that used a multi-level frequency-tuning protocol (Beetz et al. 2017).
The fact that high-frequency units in the IC also respond to low frequencies has been described before in studies in other bat species such as the mustached bat, Pteronotus parnellii (Mittmann and Wenstrup 1995; Portfors and Wenstrup 1999; Macías et al. 2012). It appears that in some bat species, the tonotopic representation in the IC differs from the classical view so that, superimposed on the canonical dorso–ventral, low- to high-frequency axis, there is responsivity to low frequencies among the high-frequency region. In P. parnellii, multi-peaked frequency tuning is especially useful for integrating information about biosonar call and echoes in different frequency channels during target distance calculations (Suga and O’Neill 1979; O’Neill and Suga 1982; Mittmann and Wenstrup 1995; Portfors and Wenstrup 1999; Wenstrup et al. 1999; Macías et al. 2012). However, C. perspicillata (the species studied here) does not use multiple frequency channels for target distance calculations (Hagemann et al. 2010; Hechavarría et al. 2013; Kössl et al. 2014). Consequently, in this species, multi-peaked frequency tuning could offer advantages for representing communication sounds (i.e., distress) in widespread neuronal populations (Kanwal et al. 1994). Note that our data suggest that ventral IC neurons provide the highest information about distress and echolocation sequences. However, the latter does not necessarily imply that dorsal IC areas do not respond to some of the features in the natural sounds. Furthermore, delay-tuned neurons are located mostly in ventral IC regions (Wenstrup and Portfors 2011; Wenstrup et al. 2012). Whether delay tuning plays a role in representing communication sounds needs to be addressed in future studies.
According to our data, in the IC, neuronal information about natural acoustic sequences increase in an exponential continuum along the dorso–ventral axis. However, our study focused on the ability of neurons to represent information using a rate code. We did not quantify information using temporal codes. Future studies could clarify whether temporal and rate codes provide similar information patterns across IC depths. At least with the rate code, ventral IC units convey the most information about both echolocation and communication calls. This was an unexpected result due to the spectral differences between echolocation and communication calls and the tonotopic organization of the IC. Ventral IC neurons can provide informative responses about distress sounds because of several reasons. (1) A large proportion of ventral neurons respond to both low and high frequencies (i.e., multi-peaked frequency tuning), (2) the fact that distress calls carry energy at both low and high frequencies (albeit having their peak energy at ~ 22 kHz), and (3) a combination of both the former and latter. In other words, in multi-peaked units, the arrival of distress calls could activate inputs that correspond to both the low- and high-frequency peaks in the tuning curves. Note, however, that the amount of information provided by single- and double-peaked units in the ventral IC does not differ (Fig. S2), indicating that high information values is not solely correlated with the presence/absence of multi-peaked tuning.
Complex receptive fields could be beneficial for natural sound tracking because of several reasons. First, in response to distress, the simultaneous arrival of low- and high-frequency driven excitatory inputs would lead to spatiotemporal summation, which ultimately transduces into stronger responses (Magee 2000). Another possibility that could be considered is that in response to distress sounds, adaptation in the low- and high-frequency synapses occurs in an asynchronous manner. In the latter scenario, ventral IC neurons would always receive excitatory inputs since adaptation alternates between low- and high-frequency information channels. Note, however, that the data gathered using echolocation sequences do not differ much from that gathered using distress sequences (Fig. 4). Echolocation does not carry strong energy at frequencies below 45 kHz and the latter suggest that high-frequency inputs are sufficient for driving highly informative responses in the ventral IC. One could argue that echolocation requires specializations for precise temporal processing (Neuweiler 1990; Wenstrup and Portfors 2011; Kössl et al. 2014) and bats may profit from these adaptations even when listening to communication sounds. From the predictive coding framework (Remez et al. 1981; Bastos et al. 2012; Ayala et al. 2016), one could argue that echolocation processing implies a low-weighted prediction error which follows a “prior” hardwired in the system. In C. perspicillata such neural prior occurs in the form of a good sound tracking ability in ventral IC areas. Communication-sound processing would benefit also from this innate high informative prior.
Note that our data offer only insights into the final activity output of IC units but is not suited for assessing which of the above explanations (if any) contributes to the improved information representation in ventral IC layers. Information estimates used here only quantify the abilities of neurons to encode acoustic inputs, yet they do not capture the parameters of the stimulus the neurons are sensitive to (Borst and Theunisse 1999; Chechik et al. 2006; Timme and Lapish 2018). Thus, although both types of stimuli (distress and echolocation) showed similar patterns of information representation throughout the IC, different parameters of the two call types could contribute in different extents to the information maps observed.
Possible origins of redundant information representation in the ventral IC
We observed that neurons in the ventral IC have complex receptive fields and carry high information content about natural sound sequences. However, the ventral IC also provides the most redundant information representations between units studied simultaneously. We show that redundancy in the ventral IC is linked to signal correlations, i.e., stimulus-induced activity correlations that arise when receptive fields overlap at least partially (Latham and Nirenber 2005; Averbeck et al. 2006). Common synaptic inputs can introduce both signal and noise correlations and could be the origin for the redundancy values reported here. In the cat’s IC, signal correlations have also been reckoned as the main contributor to the redundancy, which in turn decreases at higher stations of the auditory pathway (Chechik et al. 2006).
Our data indicate that in the IC, signal correlations are stronger than noise correlations, but this does not imply the absence of the latter. Signal correlations could relate to shared feedforward projections that dominate spiking during stimulus-driven activity and to both the crossed projections from the contralateral IC and local connections. On the other hand, noise correlations can arise from common input as well, in combination with stimulus-independent neuromodulation acting on each neuron individually (Belitski et al. 2010). Noise correlations could also reflect feedback projections, e.g., from the auditory cortex (Jen et al. 1998; Yan and Sug 1998) or the amygdala (Marsh et al. 2002), and they could regulate the IC’s processing at the single neuron level. In addition, the IC receives crossed projections from the contralateral IC and has a dense network of intrinsic connections (Malmierca et al. 1995) that could also influence the information interactions.
In the present study, we report high redundancy levels in pairs formed by close-by neurons (~ < 400 µm apart). This fact can be explained by the common inputs to neighbor neurons. Such common inputs might result from the IC’s tonotopy and they minimize wiring costs, an evolutionary adaptation linked to the formation of topographic maps (Chklovskii and Koulakov 2001, 2004). In the bat IC, there are also “synergistic neuronal pairs”, although consistent with the previous literature (Samonds et al. 2004; Narayanan et al. 2005), the predominant form of information interaction is redundancy. Studies have argued that the main advantage of redundant information regimes is that multiple copies of essentially the same information exist in the neural network activity patterns, i.e., similar information channels exist (Pitkow and Angelaki 2017). The latter gives room to the implementation of computationally different transformations on each information channel. Such transformations might be used by the bat auditory system to extract relevant stream features that go beyond the representation of the sounds’ envelope (e.g., occurrence of bouts in distress sequences or precise coding of echo-delays (Beetz et al. 2016a; García-Rosales et al. 2018)) in higher-order structures of the auditory hierarchy.
Materials and methods
Animals
For this study, four adult animals (three males, species C. perspicillata) were used. The animals were taken from the bat colony at the Institute for Cell Biology and Neuroscience at the Goethe University in Frankfurt am Main, Germany. The experiments comply with all current German laws on animal experimentation. All experimental protocols were approved by the Regierungspräsidium Darmstadt, permit #FU-1126.
Surgical procedures
On the day of the surgery, the bats were caught at the colony and were anesthetized subcutaneously with a mixture of ketamine (10 mg/kg Ketavet, Pharmacia GmbH, Germany) and xylazine (38 mg/kg Rompun, Bayer Vital GmbH, Germany). Local anesthesia (ropivacaine hydrochloride, 2 mg/ml, Fresenius Kabi, Germany) was applied subcutaneously on the skin covering the skull. Under deep anesthesia, the skin in the dorsal part of the head was cut and removed, together with the muscle tissue that covers the dorsal and temporal regions of the skull. For fixation of the bat’s head during neurophysiology measurements, a custom-made metal rod (1-cm long, 0.1 cm diameter) was glued onto the skull using acrylic glue (Heraeus Kulzer GmbH), super glue (UHU) and dental cement (Paladur, Heraeus Kulzer GmbH, Germany). A craniotomy was performed 2–3 mm lateral from the midline above the lambdoid suture on the left hemisphere using a scalpel blade. The brain surface exposed was ~ 1 mm2.
During the surgery and the recordings, the custom-made bat holder was kept at 28º C with the aid of a heating pad. The surgery was performed on day 0, and the first recording was (at the earliest) on day 2. Further recordings were performed on non-consecutive days. On each experimental day, experiments did not last longer than 4 h, and during the recordings the animals received water every ~ 1.5 h. The animals participated in the experiments for a maximum of 14 days. After this time period, they were euthanized with an anesthetic overdose (0.1 ml pentobarbital, 160 mg/ml, Narcoren, Boehringer Ingelheim Vetmedica GmbH, Germany).
Electrophysiological recordings
All recordings were performed in an electrically shielded and sound-proofed Faraday cage. Each recording consisted of three protocols: iso-level frequency tuning, spontaneous activity measurements and natural calls (three distress and one echolocation). In each recording day, the bat was placed on the holder and the rod on its skull was fixated to avoid head movements. Ropivacaine (2 mg/ml, Fresenius Kabi, Germany) was applied topically whenever wounds were handled.
On the first recording day, a small hole in the skull was made for the reference and ground electrodes on the right hemisphere in a non-auditory area. The same electrode was used for these two purposes by short-circuiting their connectors. The recording electrode (A16, NeuroNexus, Ann Arbor, MI), was an iridium laminar probe containing 16 channels arranged vertically with 50-µm inter-channel distance, 1.1–1.4 MΩ impendence, 15-µm thickness, and a 50-µm space between the tip and the first channel. The electrode was introduced 2–3 mm laterally from the scalp midline, ~ 1 mm caudal to the lambdoid suture (Coleman and Clerici 1987; Beetz et al. 2017), and perpendicularly to the surface of the brain, with the aid of a Piezo manipulator (PM-101, Science products GmbH, Hofheim, Germany). Before starting the recordings, the electrode was lowered down by 1.1–2.8 mm (depth measured from electrode’s tip to the brain’s surface). The tip’s depth was used as a reference to calculate the depth of all the channels. The position of the inferior colliculus was assessed by examining the responsivity to sounds across all recording channels. The sound used for testing for acoustic responsiveness was a short-broadband distress syllable covering frequencies between 10 and 80 kHz. The electrophysiological signals obtained were amplified (USB-ME16-FAI-System, Multi Channel Systems MCS GmbH, Germany) and stored in a computer using a sampling frequency of 25 kHz. The data were stored and monitored on-line in MC-Rack (version 4.6.2, Multi Channel Systems MCS GmbH, Germany).
Acoustic stimulation
For the present study, three types of acoustic stimuli were used: pure tones, natural echolocation calls and distress calls from conspecifics. To assess the tonotopic arrangement of the IC, pure tones (10 ms duration, 0.5 ms rise/fall time) were presented at frequencies from 10 to 90 kHz in steps of 5 kHz at a fixed level of 60-dB SPL. The 17 pure-tone stimuli were played in a pseudo-random manner with a total of 20 repetitions per sound. Note that since the mammalian cochlear frequency-place map is logarithmic (Békésy 1960) and the pure tones used here are linearly spaced, the responses to low-frequency stimuli could have been undersampled.
The natural sounds comprised three distress and one biosonar sequences. The distress calls used as stimuli were recorded from conspecifics in the context of a previous study (see (Hechavarría et al. 2016a) for description of the procedures), and the echolocation call was recorded using a pendulum setup as in (Beetz et al. 2016b), which contains echoes at delays from 23 to 1 ms. These stimuli (also referred in this manuscript as Seq1, Seq2, Seq3 and Echo) had durations of 1.51, 2.47, 2.93 and 1.38 s, respectively. Each stimulus was played 50 times in a pseudo-random order. The root-mean-square level of the syllables that formed the sequences spanned between 74.5- and 93.1-dB SPL (Table 1). The sequences were multiplied at the beginning and end by a linear fading window (10 ms length) to avoid acoustic artifacts. Sounds were played from a sound card (ADI-2-Pro, RME, Germany) at a sampling rate of 192 kHz, connected to a power amplifier (Rotel RA-12 Integrated Amplifier, Japan) and to a speaker (NeoX 1.0 True Ribbon Tweeter; Fountek Electronics, China) placed 30 cm away from the right ear. The speaker was calibrated using a microphone (¼-inch Microphone Brüel & Kjær, model 4135) recorded at 16 bit and 384 kHz of sampling frequency with a microphone amplifier (Nexus 2690, Brüel & Kjær). The resulting calibration curve is found in Fig. S1.
Spike detection and sorting
Spikes were identified after filtering the data using a third-order Butterworth band-pass filter with cutoff frequencies of 300 Hz and 3 kHz. The threshold for spike detection was 6 MAD (median absolute deviation). Spikes were sorted using the open-source algorithm SpyKING CIRCUS (Yger et al. 2018), a method that relies on density-based clustering and template matching, and can assign spikes clusters to individual channels in electrode arrays without cluster overlap. For further analysis, the cluster with the largest number of spikes was used for each channel. This spike-sorting algorithm ensures that the same cluster is not considered in different channels. Spike-sorted responses are referred to as “units” throughout the manuscript.
Information theoretic analyses
All the information theoretic analyses were performed using the Information Breakdown Toolbox (ibTB) (Magri et al. 2009). The capability of a neuron with a set of responses R to encode a set of stimuli S can be quantified using Shannon’s mutual information (I(R;S)) (Shannon 1948) using the following equation:
where P[s] is the probability of presenting the stimulus s, P[r] is the probability of observing the spike count r and P[r,s] is the joint probability of presenting the stimulus s and observing the response r. The units of the mutual information are given in bits (when the base of the logarithm is 2). Each bit implies a reduction of the uncertainty about the stimulus by a factor of 2 by observing a single trial (Dayan and Abbot 2001). One variable provides information about another variable when knowledge of the first, on average, reduces the uncertainty in the second (Cover and Thomas 2006). Mutual information provides advantages in comparison to other methods as it is model independent and thus it is not necessary to hypothesize the type of interactions between the variables studied (Magri et al. 2009; Timme and Lapish 2018) and captures all nonlinear dependencies in any statistical order (Kayser et al. 2009).
The naturalistic stimuli presented here were chunked into non-overlapping time windows, the neuronal responses to which were used to estimate the information, as it has been similarly done and described in other studies (Steveninck et al. 1997; Belitski et al. 2008; Montemurro et al. 2008; Kayser et al. 2009; García-Rosales et al. 2018). The time window considered here for the substimuli (T = 4 ms) has been selected to make our calculations comparable to those from studies in the AC (Kayser et al. 2009; García-Rosales et al. 2018). To calculate the information contained in the firing rate of each unit (Irate), the number of spikes that occurred in response (r) to each substimulus (sk) was determined. The responses were binarized, i.e., they show if there is at least one spike (1) or none (0), r = {0, 1}. P(r) indicates the probability of firing (or not) and was estimated considering all the 50 trials of each sequence.
Information was quantified by two main neuronal codes: the rate code (Irate) and the information carried by the rate of two units (Ijoint) recorded simultaneously. The Ijoint was calculated in the same manner as the Irate with the difference that now the response (r) can take four forms (instead of two) as it keeps track of which neuron fired, therefore r = {0–0, 0–1, 1–0, 1–1}. As mentioned in the preceding text, the spike-clustering algorithm used in this paper considers the geometry of the laminar probe. Therefore, for each recording channel, a different spike waveform is allocated. To further make sure that a unit was not paired with itself during Ijoint calculations, we considered only pairs of units from channels separated by at least a 100-μm distance. The mutual information was calculated as well for groups of three, four and five units recorded simultaneously.
To evaluate if the information carried by a unit pair was independent, redundant or synergistic, we calculated the information interaction (Ii). Ii in pairs of neurons can be quantified by the sum of information conveyed by those neurons individually (Irate_sum = Irate(a) + Irate(b)) and the difference between information conveyed by the two neurons (Ijoint) (Brenner et al. 2000; Narayanan et al. 2005; Chechik et al. 2006):
If Ijoint < Irate_sum or simply if the Ii estimate is positive, the units carry redundant information; if Ijoint = Irate_sum, the units carry independent information and if Ijoint > Irate_sum, or if the Ii estimate is negative, they carry synergistic information. This compares the information available in the joint response to the information available in the individual responses.
The Ii was broken down into two components: the effects of signal and noise correlations. The signal similarity component (Isign) quantifies the amount of information specifically due to signal correlations, i.e., the degree to which the (trial-averaged) signal changes with the stimulus (Belitski et al. 2008). Isign always reduces the Ii and typically arises when the neurons have similar tuning curves (Averbeck et al. 2006), i.e., when there are similarities between the responses of the units considered across substimuli (Magri et al. 2009). The noise correlation component (Inoise) quantifies the impact of the trial-by-trial variability and can increase the Ii, decrease it or leave it unchanged. Since it is measured at fixed substimulus, the Inoise disregards all effects attributable to shared stimulation (Magri et al. 2009):
Information estimates were calculated by the “direct” method (Borst and Theunissen 1999), which requires a large amount of experimental data as it does not make any assumption about response probability distributions. As it is very difficult and improbable to observe all possible responses from the entire response set (R) (Panzeri and Treves 1996; Strong et al. 1998) due to the lack of unlimited number of trials, the quantities calculated with the estimated probabilities will always be biased. To account for that, the ibTB toolbox (Magri et al. 2009) uses the Quadratic Extrapolation (QE) procedure (Strong et al. 1998) and the subtraction of any remaining bias by a bootstrap procedure (Montemurro et al. 2008). In addition, for the Ijoint tests, the Shuffling procedure (Panzeri et al. 2007) was also applied; which is also implemented in the ibTB toolbox.
To test the performance of the bias correction, simulated data with first-order statistics close to those of the real data were generated. For the Irate, spike responses were generated (inhomogeneous Poisson processes) with the same PSTH as each real unit used for the analysis (as in García-Rosales et al. 2018). Information was computed for the simulated data using the same parameters than for the original data, for all the neural codes used (Irate, and Ijoint for pairs, triplets, quadruplets and quintuplets) and for different number of trials (4, 8, 16, 32, 50, 64, 128, 256, 512). According to our results of the performance of the bias correction on simulated data, the bias was negligible for the Irate, Ijoint for pairs and triplets and had slightly negative for the Ijoint for quadruplets and quintuplets, the information estimates underestimate the true information values for the last two variables.
First-spike latency estimation
The first-spike latency was calculated as the time point in which the spiking rate in the observed response was significantly different from the expected spontaneous rate, assuming Poisson statistics (Chase and Young 2005), i.e., this method detects the first significant deviation from the expected spontaneous rate. For that, spikes from all N trials are collapsed (50 trials). The probability of observing a response of at least n spikes in a window tn (after stimulus onset) assuming Poisson statistics is
where \(\lambda\) is the spontaneous spike rate. Starting from the stimulus onset, the probability that each spike is the result of a stronger than chance rate deviation from the spontaneous rate (calculated during the 500 ms preceding the stimulus onset), is calculated as the probability that the spontaneous rate would have produced that spike as the last of n spikes in a window tn, where n ranges from 5 up to all the spikes considered in the particular window and tn is the width of the window containing these spikes. The first time that these probabilities exceed a threshold of 10–6 is considered the unit’s latency to that particular stimulus.
The first-spike latency was used to shift the units’ responses, i.e., the onset of the responses was at the units’ latencies, for a recalculation of the Irate (Fig. S3).
Statistics
All statistical tests were performed using custom-written Matlab scripts (R2019b, MathWorks, Natick, MA). Non-parametric Wilcoxon rank-sum tests were used to assess the statistical difference of unpaired data and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for paired data. Significant differences were considered when p < 0.05. Multiple comparisons were corrected for the false-discovery rate (FDR) using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995). In several figures, data are shown as violin plots (Hintze and Nelson 1998), which display distributions with density traces, median values with circles and interquartile ranges with thick lines. r (r = z/√N) was calculated as a non-parametric effect size measure (Fritz et al. 2012) to determine the importance of the effect in Fig. S3D.
References
Averbeck BB, Latham PE, Pouget A (2006) Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:358–366
Ayala YA, Pérez-González D, Malmierca MS (2016) Stimulus-specific adaptation in the inferior colliculus: the role of excitatory, inhibitory and modulatory inputs. Biol Psychol 116:10–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.06.016
Balcombe JP, McCracken GF (1992) Vocal recognition in mexican free-tailed bats: do pups recognize mothers? Anim Behav 43:79–87
Bastos AM, Usrey WM, Adams RA, Mangun GR, Fries P, Friston KJ (2012) Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron 76:695–711. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038
Beetz MJ, Hechavarría JC, Kössl M (2016a) Temporal tuning in the bat auditory cortex is sharper when studied with natural echolocation sequences. Sci Rep 6:29102. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep29102
Beetz MJ, Hechavarría JC, Kössl M (2016b) Cortical neurons of bats respond best to echoes from nearest targets when listening to natural biosonar multi-echo streams. Sci Rep 6:35991. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep35991
Beetz MJ, Kordes S, García-Rosales F, Kössl M, Hechavarría JC (2017) Processing of natural echolocation sequences in the inferior colliculus of Seba’s fruit eating bat. Carollia perspicillata. eNeuro 4:e0314–17. https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0314-17.2017
Békésy G Von (1960) Experiments in hearing. McGraw-Hill, New York
Belitski A, Gretton A, Magri C, Murayama Y, Montemurro MA, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S (2008) Low-frequency local field potentials and spikes in primary visual cortex convey independent visual information. J Neurosci 28:5696–5709. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0009-08.2008
Belitski A, Panzeri S, Magri C, Logothetis NK, Kayser C (2010) Sensory information in local field potentials and spikes from visual and auditory cortices: time scales and frequency bands. J Comput Neurosci 29:533–545
Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B 57:289–300
Borst A, Theunissen FE (1999) Information theory and neural coding. Nat Neurosci 2:947–957. https://doi.org/10.1038/14731
Brenner N, Strong SP, Koberle R, Bialek W, de Ruyter van Steveninck R (2000) Synergy in a neural code. Neural Comput 12:1531–1552
Brinkløv S, Jakobsen L, Ratcliffe JM, Kalko EKV, Surlykke A (2011) Echolocation call intensity and directionality in flying short-tailed fruit bats, Carollia perspicillata (Phyllostomidae). J Acoust Soc Am 129:427–435. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3519396
Brudzynski SM (2013) Ethotransmission: communication of emotional states through ultrasonic vocalization in rats. Curr Opin Neurobiol 23:310–317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.014
Casseday JH, Covey E (1992) Frequency tuning properties of neurons in the inferior colliculus of an FM bat. J Comp Neurol 319:34–50
Casseday JH, Covey E (1996) A neuroethological theory of the operation of the inferior colliculus. Brain Behav Evol 47:323–336
Casseday JH, Fremouw T, Covey E (2002) The inferior colliculus: a hub for the central auditory system. Integrative functions in the mammalian auditory pathway. Springer, New York, NY, pp 238–318
Chase SM, Young ED (2005) Limited segregation of different types of sound localization information among classes of units in the inferior colliculus. J Neurosci 25:7575–7585. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0915-05.2005
Chechik G, Anderson MJ, Bar-Yosef O, Young ED, Tishby N, Nelken I (2006) Reduction of information redundancy in the ascending auditory pathway. Neuron 51:359–368
Chklovskii DB, Koulakov AA (2001) Orientation preference patterns in mammalian visual cortex: a wire length minimization approach. Neuron 29:519–527
Chklovskii DB, Koulakov AA (2004) Maps in the brain: what can we learn from them? Annu Rev Neurosci 27:369–392
Coleman JR, Clerici WJ (1987) Sources of projections to subdivisions of the inferior colliculus in the rat. J Comp Neurol 262:215–226. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902620204
Colletti V, Shannon R, Carner M, Sacchetto L, Turazzi S, Masotto B, Colletti L (2007) The first successful case of hearing produced by electrical stimulation of the human midbrain. Otol Neurotol 28:39–43
Cover TM, Thomas JA (2006) Elements of information theory, 2nd edn. Wiley-Interscience, New York
Covey E, Hall WC, Kobler JB (1987) Subcortical connections of the superior colliculus in the mustache bat, Pteronotus parnellii. J Comp Neurol 263:179–197
Dayan P, Abbott LF (2001) Information Theory. In: Theoretical neuroscience: computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems. Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press, pp 123–135
Eckenweber M, Knörnschild M (2016) Responsiveness to conspecific distress calls is influenced by day-roost proximity in bats (Saccopteryx bilineata). R Soc Open Sci 3:160151. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160151
Fitzpatrick DC, Kanwal JS, Butman JA, Suga N (1993) Combination-sensitive neurons in the primary auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J Neurosci 13:931–940
Friauf E (1992) Tonotopic order in the adult and developing auditory system of the rat as shown by c-fos immunocytochemistry. Eur J Neurosci 4:798–812. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.1992.tb00190.x
Fritz CO, Morris PE, Richler JJ (2012) Effect size estimates: current use, calculations, and interpretation. J Exp Psychol Gen 141:2–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024338
Gadziola MA, Grimsley JMS, Faure PA, Wenstrup JJ (2012) Social vocalizations of big brown bats vary with behavioral context. PLoS One 7:e44550. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0044550
García-Rosales F, Beetz MJ, Cabral-Calderin Y, Kössl M, Hechavarria JC (2018) Neuronal coding of multiscale temporal features in communication sequences within the bat auditory cortex. Commun Biol 1:200. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-018-0205-5
Gaucher Q, Panniello M, Ivanov AZ, Dahmen JC, King AJ, Walker KMM (2020) Complexity of frequency receptive fields predicts tonotopic variability across species. eLife 9:e53462
Grinnell B (1963) The neurophysiology of audition in bats: intensity and frequency parameters. J Physiol 167:38–66
Hagemann C, Esser KH, Kössl M (2010) Chronotopically organized target-distance map in the auditory cortex of the short-tailed fruit bat. J Neurophysiol 103:322–333
Hagemann C, Vater M, Kössl M (2011) Comparison of properties of cortical echo delay-tuning in the short-tailed fruit bat and the mustached bat. J Comp Physiol A 197:605–613. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-010-0530-8
Hechavarría JC, Macías S, Vater M, Voss C, Mora EC, Kössl M (2013) Blurry topography for precise target-distance computations in the auditory cortex of echolocating bats. Nat Commun 4:2587. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms3587
Hechavarría JC, Beetz MJ, Macias S, Kössl M (2016a) Distress vocalization sequences broadcasted by bats carry redundant information. J Comp Physiol A 202:503–515. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-016-1099-7
Hechavarría JC, Beetz MJ, MacIas S, Kössl M (2016b) Vocal sequences suppress spiking in the bat auditory cortex while evoking concomitant steady-state local field potentials. Sci Rep 6:39226. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep39226
Hintze JL, Nelson RD (1998) Violin Plots: A Box Plot-Density Trace Synergism. Am Stat 52:181–184
Holmstrom L, Roberts PD, Portfors CV (2007) Responses to social vocalizations in the inferior colliculus of the mustached bat are influenced by secondary tuning curves. J Neurophysiol 98:3461–3472. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00638.2007
Jen PHS, Chen QC (1998) The effect of pulse repetition rate, pulse intensity, and bicuculline on the minimum threshold and latency of bat inferior collicular neurons. J Comp Physiol A 182:455–465
Jen PHS, Chen QC, Sun XD (1998) Corticofugal regulation of auditory sensitivity in the bat inferior colliculus. J Comp Physiol A 183:683–697
Jiang T, Huang X, Wu H, Feng J (2017) Size and quality information in acoustic signals of Rhinolophus ferrumequinum in distress situations. Physiol Behav 173:252–257
Kanwal JS, Matsumura S, Ohlemiller K, Suga N (1994) Analysis of acoustic elements and syntax in communication sounds emitted by mustached bats. J Acoust Soc Am 96:1229–1254
Kanwal JS, Rauschecker JP (2007) Auditory cortex of bats and primates: managing species-specific calls for social communication. Front Biosci 1:4621–4640
Kayser C, Montemurro MA, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S (2009) Spike-phase coding boosts and stabilizes the information carried by spatial and temporal spike patterns. Neuron 61:597–608. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.008
Knörnschild M, Feifel M, Kalko EKV (2013) Mother–offspring recognition in the bat Carollia perspicillata. Anim Behav 86:941–948
Kössl M, Hechavarria J, Voss C, Schaefer M, Vater M (2015) Bat auditory cortex - model for general mammalian auditory computation or special design solution for active time perception? Eur J Neurosci 41:518–532
Kössl M, Hechavarria JC, Voss C, Macias S, Mora EC, Vater M (2014) Neural maps for target range in the auditory cortex of echolocating bats. Curr Opin Neurobiol 24:68–75
Latham PE, Nirenberg S (2005) Synergy, redundancy, and independence in population codes, revisited. J Neurosci 25:5195–5206
Liévin-Bazin A, Pineaux M, Clerc O, Gahr M, von Bayern AMP, Bovet D (2018) Emotional responses to conspecific distress calls are modulated by affiliation in cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus). PLoS One 13:e0205314. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0205314
Lim HH, Lenarz T, Joseph G, Battmer RD, Samii A, Samii M, Patrick JF, Lenarz M (2007) Electrical stimulation of the midbrain for hearing restoration: Insight into the functional organization of the human central auditory system. J Neurosci 27:13541–13551
Lim HH, Lenarz T, Anderson DJ, Lenarz M (2008) The auditory midbrain implant: effects of electrode location. Hear Res 242:74–85
Lim HH, Lenarz M, Lenarz T (2009) Auditory midbrain implant: a review. Trends Amplif 13:149–180
López-Jury L, Mannel A, García-Rosales F, Hechavarria JC (2019) Modified synaptic dynamics predict neural activity patterns in an auditory field within the frontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci 51:1011–1025. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14600
Macías S, Luo J, Moss CF (2018) Natural echolocation sequences evoke echo-delay selectivity in the auditory midbrain of the FM bat, eptesicus fuscus. J Neurophysiol 120:1323–1339
Macías S, Mora EC, Hechavarría JC, Kössl M (2012) Properties of echo delay-tuning receptive fields in the inferior colliculus of the mustached bat. Hear Res 286:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2012.02.013
Magee JC (2000) Dendritic integration of excitatory synaptic input. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:181–190
Magri C, Whittingstall K, Singh V, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S (2009) A toolbox for the fast information analysis of multiple-site LFP, EEG and spike train recordings. BMC Neurosci 10:81. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-10-81
Malmierca MS (2004) The inferior colliculus: A center for convergence of ascending and descending auditory information. Neuroembryology Aging 3:215–229
Malmierca MS, Izquierdo MA, Cristaudo S, Hernández O, Pérez-González D, Covey E, Oliver DL (2008) A discontinuous tonotopic organization in the inferior colliculus of the rat. J Neurosci 28:4767–4776
Malmierca MS, Rees A, Le Beau FEN, Bjaalie JG (1995) Laminar organization of frequency-defined local axons within and between the inferior colliculi of the guinea pig. J Comp Neurol 357:124–144
Marsh RA, Fuzessery ZM, Grose CD, Wenstrup JJ (2002) Projection to the inferior colliculus from the basal nucleus of the amygdala. J Neurosci 22:10449–10460
Martin LM, García-Rosales F, Beetz MJ, Hechavarría JC (2017) Processing of temporally patterned sounds in the auditory cortex of Seba’s short-tailed bat, Carollia perspicillata. Eur J Neurosci 46:2365–2379. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13702
Mittmann DH, Wenstrup JJ (1995) Combination-sensitive neurons in the inferior colliculus. Hear Res 90:185–191
Montemurro MA, Rasch MJ, Murayama Y, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S (2008) Phase-of-firing coding of natural visual stimuli in primary visual cortex. Curr Biol 18:375–380
Narayanan NS, Kimchi EY, Laubach M (2005) Redundancy and synergy of neuronal ensembles in motor cortex. J Neurosci 25:4207–4216
Neuweiler G (1990) Auditory adaptations for prey capture in echolocating bats. Physiol Rev 70:615–641
Neuweiler G (2003) Evolutionary aspects of bat echolocation. J Comp Physiol A 189:245–256
O’Neill WE, Suga N (1982) Encoding of target range and its representation in the auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J Neurosci 2:17–31
Panzeri S, Treves A (1996) Analytical estimates of limited sampling biases in different information measures. Netw Comput Neural Syst 7:87–107
Panzeri S, Senatore R, Montemurro MA, Petersen RS (2007) Correcting for the sampling bias problem in spike train information measures. J Neurophysiol 98:1064–1072. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00559.2007
Pitkow X, Angelaki DE (2017) Inference in the brain: statistics flowing in redundant population codes. Neuron 94:943–953. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.028
Portfors CV, Wenstrup JJ (1999) Delay-tuned neurons in the inferior colliculus of the mustached bat: implications for analyses of target distance. J Neurophysiol 82:1326–1338
Remez R, Rubin P, Pisoni D, Carrell T (1981) Speech perception without traditional speech cues. Science 212:947–949
Russ J, Jones G, Mackie I, Racey P (2004) Interspecific responses to distress calls in bats (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae): a function for convergence in call design? Anim Behav 67:1005–1014
Russ JM, Racey PA, Jones G (1998) Intraspecific responses to distress calls of the pipistrelle bat, Pipistrellus pipistrellus. Anim Behav 55:705–713
Ryan JM, Clark DB, Lackey JA (1985) Response of Artibeus lituratus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae) to distress calls of conspecifics. J Mammal 66:179–181. https://doi.org/10.2307/1380980
Samonds JM, Allison JD, Brown HA, Bonds AB (2004) Cooperative synchronized assemblies enhance orientation discrimination. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:6722–6727
Schnitzler H-U, Moss CF, Denzinger A (2003) From spatial orientation to food acquisition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol Evol 18:386–394. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(03)00185-X
Shannon CE (1948) A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst Tech J 27:379–423
Simmons JA, Yashiro H, Kohler AL, Riquimaroux H, Funabiki K, Simmons AM (2020) Long-latency optical responses from the dorsal inferior colliculus of Seba’s fruit bat. J Comp Physiol A 206:831–844. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-020-01441-7
Van Steveninck RRDR, Lewen GD, Strong SP, Koberle R, Bialek W (1997) Reproducibility and variability in neural spike trains. Science 275:1805–1808
Strong SP, Koberle R, Van Steveninck RRDR, Bialek W (1998) Entropy and information in neural spike trains. Phys Rev Lett 80:197–200
Suga N, O’Neill WE (1979) Neural axis representing target range in the auditory cortex of the mustached bat. Science 206:351–353
Sutter ML, Schreiner CE (1991) Physiology and topography of neurons with multipeaked tuning curves in cat primary auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 65:1207–1226. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1991.65.5.1207
Thies W, Kalko EKV, Schnitzler HU (1998) The roles of echolocation and olfaction in two Neotropical fruit-eating bats, Carollia perspicillata and C. castanea, feeding on Piper. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:397–409
Timme NM, Lapish C (2018) A tutorial for information theory in neuroscience. eNeuro 5:ENEURO.0052-18.2018. https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0052-18.2018
Wenstrup JJ, Portfors CV (2011) Neural processing of target distance by echolocating bats: functional roles of the auditory midbrain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35:2073–2083
Wenstrup JJ, Mittmann DH, Grose CD (1999) Inputs to combination-sensitive neurons of the inferior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 409:509–528
Wenstrup JJ, Nataraj K, Sanchez JT (2012) Mechanisms of spectral and temporal integration in the mustached bat inferior colliculus. Front Neural Circuits 6:75. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2012.00075
Wilkinson GS, Boughman JW (1998) Social calls coordinate foraging in greater spear-nosed bats. Anim Behav 55:337–350
Wohlgemuth MJ, Moss CF (2016) Midbrain auditory selectivity to natural sounds. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:2508–2513
Yan W, Suga N (1998) Corticofugal modulation of the midbrain frequency map in the bat auditory system. Nat Neurosci 1:54–58
Yger P, Spampinato GL, Esposito E, Lefebvre B, Deny S, Gardella C, Stimberg M, Jetter F, Zeck G, Picaud S, Duebel J, Marre O (2018) A spike sorting toolbox for up to thousands of electrodes validated with ground truth recordings in vitro and in vivo. eLife 7:e34518
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Gisa Prange for help with histological staining.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This work has been funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) with the Grant number # KO 987/12–2.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
J.C.H., F.G.R., L.L.J. and E.G.P. designed the research; E.G.P. performed the research; E.G.P. and F.G.R. analyzed the data; E.G.P. and J.C.H. wrote the original draft; J.C.H., F.G.R. and L.L.J. reviewed and edited the manuscript.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Ethical approval
All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed.
Data availability statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
González-Palomares, E., López-Jury, L., García-Rosales, F. et al. Enhanced representation of natural sound sequences in the ventral auditory midbrain. Brain Struct Funct 226, 207–223 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02188-2
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02188-2
Keywords
- Inferior colliculus
- Auditory midbrain
- Mutual information
- Natural sounds
- Brain-stimulus synchrony