Skip to main content
Log in

Length perception of horizontal and vertical bisected lines

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research PRPF Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In an inverted T figure, the vertical line is largely overestimated (Avery and Day in J Exp Psychol 81:376–380, 1969). This vertical overestimation results from the vertical and bisection biases. Line orientation biases length perception in the sense that the vertical line of a L shape is perceived as longer than the horizontal line of the same physical length. In the inverted T figure, the vertical line is overestimated because of its orientation but also because the horizontal line is bisected. In the current study, we used various two-line configurations to investigate the role of bisection a/symmetry in line length perception and its interaction with the vertical bias. Experiment 1 showed that symmetry and asymmetry of bisection have different consequences on line length perception, as previously shown by Wolfe et al. (Percept Psychophys 67:967–979, 2005). Experiments 2 focused on the relation between the vertical and bisection biases by manipulating orthogonally line orientation and bisection a/symmetry. The results provided evidence that bisection can prevent the manifestation of the vertical bias, so that when the two lines are bisected, vertical lines are not anymore overestimated. These results are discussed in the light of recent findings claiming that saccades could play an essential role in length perception.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Avery, G., & Day, R. (1969). Basis of the horizontal vertical illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 376–380. doi:10.1037/h0027737.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosvic, G. M., & Cohen, B. D. (1988). The horizontal vertical illusion and knowledge of results. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 67, 463–469.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carrasco, M., Talgar, C., & Cameron, E. L. (2001). Characterizing visual performance fields: Effects of transient covert attention, spatial frequency, eccentricity, task and set size. Spatial Vision, 14, 61–75. doi:10.1163/15685680152692015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579–593. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coren, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1978). Seeing is deceiving: The psychology of visual illusions. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (2002). Overestimation of heights in virtual reality is influenced more by perceived distal size than by 2-D versus 3-D dimensionality of the display. Perception, 31, 103–112. doi:10.1068/p3053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fick, A. (1851). De errore quodam optico asymetrica bulbi effecto. Marburg: J. A. Kochin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finger, F. W., & Spelt, D. K. (1947). The illustration of the horizontal vertical illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, 243–250. doi:10.1037/h0055605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentaz, E., & Hatwell, Y. (2004). Geometrical haptic illusions: The role of exploration in the Müller-Lyer, vertical horizontal, and Delboeuf illusions. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 31–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Girgus, J. S., & Coren, S. (1975). Depth cues and constancy scaling in the horizontal vertical illusion: The bisection error. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 29, 59–65. doi:10.1037/h0082021.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, A. L., & Slotnick, B. M. (1995). Visual inspection alone produces a decrement in the horizontal vertical illusion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81, 323–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, R. L. (1963). Distortion of visual space as inappropriate constancy scaling. Nature, 199, 678–680. doi:10.1038/199678a0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, R. L. (1970). The intelligent eye. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, K. M., & Slotnick, B. (1996). The horizontal vertical illusion: Evidence for strategic factors in feedback-induced illusion decrement. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 79–87.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Künnapas, T. M. (1955a). An analysis of the “vertical horizontal illusion”. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 134–140. doi:10.1037/h0045229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Künnapas, T. M. (1955b). Influence of frame size on apparent length of a line. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 168–170. doi:10.1037/h0044569.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Künnapas, T. M. (1957a). Vertical horizontal illusion and surrounding field. Acta Psychologica, 13, 35–42. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(57)90004-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Künnapas, T. M. (1957b). The vertical horizontal illusion and the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 405–407. doi:10.1037/h0048637.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Künnapas, T. M. (1958). Influence of head inclination on the vertical horizontal illusion. The Journal of Psychology, 46, 179–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipshits, M., & McIntyre, J. (1999). Gravity affects the preferred vertical and horizontal in visual perception of orientation. Neuroreport, 10, 1085–1089. doi:10.1097/00001756-199904060-00033.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lipshits, M., McIntyre, J., Zaoui, M., Gurfinkel, V., & Berthoz, A. (2001). Does gravity play an essential role in the asymmetrical visual perception of vertical and horizontal line length? Acta Astronautica, 49, 123–130. doi:10.1016/S0094-5765(01)00088-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCourt, M. E., & Olafson, C. (1997). Cognitive and perceptual influences in visual line bisection: Psychophysical and chronometric analyses of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 35, 369–380. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00143-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., Bradshaw, J. L., & Mattingley, J. B. (2002). Free-viewing perceptual asymmetries for the judgement of brightness, numerosity and size. Neuropsychologia, 37, 307–314. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00074-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., Mattingley, J. B., Berberovic, N., Smith, A., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2004). An investigation of the relationship between free-viewing perceptual asymmetries for vertical and horizontal stimuli. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 19, 289–301. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.12.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oppel, J. J. (1855). Über geometrisch-optische Täuschungen. Jahresbericht des Frankfurter Vereins, 3, 7–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, M. H., & Edelman, J. A. (2008). The dependence of visual scanning performance on search direction and difficulty. Vision Research, 48, 2184–2192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prinzmetal, W., & Gettleman, L. (1993). Horizontal vertical illusion: One eye is better than two. Perception and Psychophysics, 53, 81–88.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raudsepp, J., & Djupsjöbacka, M. (2005). Handgrip maximum force and the visual horizontal vertical illusion. Perception, 34, 421–428. doi:10.1068/p5073.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Renier, L., Bruyer, R., & De Volder, A. G. (2006). Vertical-horizontal illusion present for sighted but not early blind humans using auditory substitution of vision. Perception and Psychophysics, 68, 535–542.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, H. O., Wennberg, P., & Raudsepp, J. (2007). The effects of inverting prisms on the horizontal-vertical illusion: A systematic effect of downward gaze. Experimental Brain Research, 183, 9–15. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-1015-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime user’s guide. Pittsburg: Psychology Software Tools Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsal, Y., & Shalev, L. (1996). Inattention magnifies perceived length: The attentional receptive field hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 233–243. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.22.1.233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsal, Y., Meiran, N., & Lamy, D. (1995). Towards a resolution theory of attention. Visual Cognition, 2, 313–330. doi:10.1080/13506289508401736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsal, Y., Shalev, L., & Zakay, D. (2005). The lengthening effect revisited: A reply to Prinzmetal and Wilson (1997) and Masin (1999). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 185–190.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vishton, P. M., Rea, J. G., Cutting, J. E., & Nunez, L. N. (1999). Comparing effects of the horizontal vertical illusion on grip scaling and judgment: Relative versus absolute, not perception versus action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1659–1672. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1659.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • von Collani, G. (1985). Retinal projection or size constancy as determinants of the horizontal vertical illusion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 61, 547–557.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 483–488. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, P. A., & Enns, J. T. (1996). Pictorial depth and framing have independent effects on the horizontal vertical illusion. Perception, 25, 921–926. doi:10.1068/p250921.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, U., & Amis, E. (2006). Is active drawing of line configurations resistant to visual illusions. Journal of Vision (Charlottesville, Va.), 6, 355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, U., Maloney, L. T., & Tam, M. (2005). Distortions of perceived length in the frontoparallel plane: Tests of perspective theories. Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 967–979.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R. S. (1938). Experimental psychology. New York: Henry Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1897). Outlines of Psychology. Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yang, T. L., Dixon, M. W., & Profitt, D. R. (1999). Seeing big things: Overestimation of heights is greater for real objects than for objects in pictures. Perception, 28, 445–467. doi:10.1068/p2854.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Spanish “Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia” (predoctoral grant -AP-2006-3911- to the first author, and a research grant -PSI2008-03595PSIC- to the second author).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Pom Charras.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Charras, P., Lupiáñez, J. Length perception of horizontal and vertical bisected lines. Psychological Research 74, 196–206 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0243-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0243-1

Keywords

Navigation