Skip to main content
Log in

Dissociating cognitive and motor interference effects on kinesthetic short-term memory

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research PRPF Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In two experiments, we investigated how short-term memory of kinesthetically defined spatial locations suffers from either motor or cognitive distraction. In Exp. 1, 22 blindfolded participants moved a handle with their right hand towards a mechanical stop and back to the start and then reproduced the encoded stop position by a second movement. The retention interval was adjusted to approximately 0 and 8 s. In half of the trials participants had to provide a verbal judgment of the target distance after encoding (cognitive distractor). Analyses of constant and variable errors indicated that the verbal judgments interfered with the motor reproduction only, when the retention interval was long. In Exp. 2, 22 other participants performed the same task but instead of providing verbal distance estimations they performed an additional movement either with their right or left hand during the retention interval. Constant error was affected by the side of the interpolated movement (right vs. left hand) and by the delay interval. The results show that reproduction of kinesthetically encoded spatial locations is affected differently in long- and short-retention intervals by cognitive and motor interference. This suggests that reproduction behavior is based on distinct codes during immediate vs. delayed recall.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We report uncorrected degrees of freedom and corrected p values in the “Results” section.

  2. In similar experimental setups, we found that also the standard deviation, i.e. a measure not relativized to the mean amplitude, did not reflect the typically observed increase in variability with distance. In contrast, the pattern resembled the reported result, i.e. increasing distance was associated with a decrease in response variability (see Kirsch et al., 2008).

References

  • Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 701–725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw, M. F., & Watt, S. J. (2002). A dissociation of perception and action in normal human observers: the effect of temporal delay. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1766–1778.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman, B., Gemmer, A., Forsman, T., & Huemer, V. (2000). Processing spatial information in the sensorimotor branch of the visual system. Vision Research, 40, 3539–3552.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diewert, G. L. (1975). Retention and coding in motor short-term memory: A comparison of storage codes for distance and location information. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 183–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, D., & Madalena, J. (1987). The influence of premovement visual information on manual aiming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 541–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan, J. R., & Beltzner, M. A. (2000). Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size-weight illusion. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 737–741.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box’s results on the use of the F distribution in multivariate analysis. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29, 885–891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale, M. A., Jakobson, L. S., & Keillor, J. M. (1994). Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia, 32, 1159–1178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathway for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 1, 20–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale, M. A., Westwood, D. A., & Milner, A. (2004). Two distinct modes of control for object-directed action. Progress in Brain Research, 144, 131–144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grandy, M. S., & Westwood, D. A. (2006). Opposite perceptual and sensorimotor responses to a size-weight illusion. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 3887–3892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Granit, R. (1972). Constant errors in the execution and appreciation of movement. Brain, 95, 649–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagman, J. D. (1978). Specific-cue effects of interpolated movements on distance and location retention in short-term memory. Memory and Cognition, 6, 432–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J. A., Harris, I. M., & Diamond, M. E. (2001). Topography of tactile working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 8262–8269.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, Y., Eagleson, R., & Goodale, M. A. (1999). The effects of delay on the kinematics of grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 126, 109–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M. A., Rizzolatti, G., & Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends in Neurosciences, 18, 314–320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, L. A. (1986). Perception of force and weight: theory and research. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 29–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsch, W., Hennighausen, E., & Rösler, F. (2008). Electrophysiological markers of encoding and error processing in a motor matching task (in preparation).

  • Laabs, G. J. (1973). Retention characteristics of different reproduction cues in motor short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 168–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laabs, G. J. (1974). The effect of interpolated motor activity on the short-term retention of movement distance and end-location. Journal of Motor Behavior, 6, 279–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laabs, G. J. (1976). A note concerning the effect of a kinesthetic memory load on the retention of movement end-location. Journal of Motor Behavior, 8, 313–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laabs, G. J., & Simmons, R. W. (1981). Motor memory. In D. H. Holding (Ed.), Human skills (pp. 119–152). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laszlo, J. I. (1992). Motor control and learning: how far do the experimental tasks restrict our theoretical insight? In J. J. Summers (Ed.), Approaches to the study of motor control and learning (pp. 47–79). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D. J., Ellis, R. R., Bandomir, C. A., & Ross, H. E. (1999). Charpentier (1891) on the size-weight illusion. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1681–1685.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D., & Gopher, D. (1979). On the economy of the human-processing system. Psychological Review, 86, 214–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, J. (2002). Two visual systems and two theories of perception: An attempt to reconcile the constructivist and ecological approaches. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 73–144.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paillard, J. (1987). Cognitive versus sensori-motor encoding of spatial information. In P. Ellen & C. Thinus-Blanc (Eds.), Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animal and man (pp. 43–77). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1967). Characteristics of visual and kinesthetic memory codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 103–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rossetti, Y. (1998). Implicit short-lived motor representation of space in brain-damaged and healthy subjects. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 520–558.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rossetti, Y., & Pisella, L. (2002). Several vision for action systems: a guide to dissociating and integrating dorsal and ventral functions. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 62–119). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., & Boisson, D. (1995). Implicit processing of somaesthetic information: a dissociation between where and how? Neuroreport, 6, 506–510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smyth, M. M. (1984). Memory for movements. In M. M. Smyth & A. Wing (Eds.), The psychology of human movement (pp. 83–117). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soechting, J. F., & Flanders, M. (1992). Moving in three-dimensional space: Frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 167–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tillery, S. I., Flanders, M., & Soechting, J. F. (1994). Errors in kinesthetic transformations for hand apposition. Neuroreport, 6, 177–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trumbo, D., Milone, F., & Noble, M. (1972). Interpolated activity and response mechanisms in motor short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 205–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vaillancourt, D. E., & Russell, D. M. (2002). Temporal capacity of short-term visuomotor memory in continuous force production. Experimental Brain Research, 145, 275–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–1882.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wulf, G., McNevin, N., & Shea, C. H. (2001). The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, A, 54, 1143–1154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Waldemar Kirsch or Frank Rösler.

Additional information

This work was supported by the graduate school “neural representation and action control”, (Graduiertenkolleg 885) and grant Ro529/18-1, both provided by the German Research Foundation (DFG).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kirsch, W., Hennighausen, E. & Rösler, F. Dissociating cognitive and motor interference effects on kinesthetic short-term memory. Psychological Research 73, 380–389 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0159-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0159-1

Keywords

Navigation