Introduction

In recent years direct measurements of cone pigments combined with inferences drawn from examinations of the genes coding for photopigment opsins have provided a greatly enhanced understanding of the distribution and evolution of cone photopigments. The consensus is that four families of genes specify the opsin proteins in all vertebrate cone pigments (Yokoyama 2000, 2008; Hisatomi and Tokunaga 2002). Eutherian mammals have retained representatives of only two of these—SWS1 genes linked to pigments with peak absorbance (λ max) in the ultraviolet (UV) and short-wavelength (S) portions of the visible spectrum (360–445 nm) and LWS genes that yield pigments with λ max in the middle to long wavelengths (M/L 500–565 nm). Most species of this group derive one type of cone pigment from each of these two classes providing the potential for a dichromatic color vision capacity (Jacobs 2009). There are notable exceptions to this arrangement among the primates, a group in which LWS opsin gene duplications and polymorphisms have allowed many lineages to gain a total of three cone photopigments and from these derive trichromacy (Jacobs 2008).

There has also been progress toward understanding the cone pigments of protherian and metatherian mammals. Among the handful of contemporary monotremes, two cone opsin gene families have been detected in both platypus (Ornithorhynchus) and echidna (Tachyglossus) (Davies et al. 2007; Wakefield et al. 2008) The opsin genes in these animals are drawn from the SWS2 and LWS groups and yield cone pigments with respective λ max values of 450 and 550 nm. The cone pigment arrangements of marsupials are more varied. Some species of Australian marsupials have two classes of cone pigments, while others have three (Hemmi 1999; Hemmi et al. 2000; Arrese et al. 2002, 2005). Among the examples of latter is the fat-tailed dunnart (Sminthopsis carssicaudatus). Direct pigment measurements in this animal revealed three types of cone pigment and subsequent behavioral measurements have verified the presence of a trichromatic color vision capacity (Arrese et al. 2006). However, whereas two of these cone pigment types are representatives of the SWS1 and LWS gene families, there is no direct evidence that the third cone pigment (λ max of ~500 nm) in fact derives from a cone opsin gene (Strachan et al. 2004; Cowing et al. 2008). To this point the gene/cone pigment linkage in this marsupial therefore remains somewhat puzzling, but one possibility is that the third type of cone in this animal contains a rod photopigment.

Roughly one-third of all marsupial species are found in the Americas. A vast majority of these are native to South America and recent examination of the opsin genes in two species from this group (Didelphis auria, the big-eared opossum, and Monodelphis domestica, the gray short-tailed opossum) detected opsin genes for two types of cone photopigment drawn, respectively, from SWS1 and LWS gene families (Hunt et al. 2009). Opossums are believed to have migrated northward from South America across the Isthmus of Panama perhaps about 3 million years ago (Macdonald 2001). Only a single species, the Virginia opossum (D. virginiana), is found in North America where it currently occupies a range that runs northward from Mexico to the Canadian border encompassing much of the eastern two-thirds of the United States as well as the western states bordering the Pacific ocean. This species is stringently nocturnal and in accord with that behavior the retina contains only a sparse population of retinal cones (Walls 1939; Wikler and Rakic 1990; Kolb and Wang 1985). Over the years, we have employed a gross potential electrophysiological technique to attempt to access cone signals in the retina of this species (some early efforts were noted in Jacobs 1993). Because of the current interest in understanding the nature of marsupial cone pigments, a brief summary of the more recent results from these efforts is provided here.

Materials and methods

Adult Virginia opossums (D. virginiana) of both sexes were trapped in Santa Barbara County, California under the auspices of a scientific collecting permit issued by the State of California, Department of Fish and Game. All animal care and experimental procedures were in accordance with institutional animal care and use guidelines and with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (National Academy Press, 1996).

In preparation for recording the opossums were anesthetized with a mixture of xylazine hydrochloride (8 mg kg−1) and ketamine hydrochloride (42 mg kg−1) injected intramuscularly followed by intraperitoneal administration of pentobarbital sodium (50 mg kg−1). The pupil of one eye was dilated by topical application of atropine sulfate (0.04%). The animal was positioned in a head holder and aligned with a three-beam Maxwellian view optical system that images a 59° circular field on the retina. Hypothermia has been reported to be a major side effect of anesthesia in opossums (Keller et al. 1988) so throughout the recording session body temperature was supported with the use of a circulating hot water heater. The electroretinogram (ERG) was differentially recorded using a contact lens electrode. To enhance the likelihood of eliciting contributions from cones the recording sessions were conducted in a room that was ambiently illuminated to yield ~150 lux at the cornea of the eye being tested.

For recording we employed ERG flicker photometry, a technique frequently used to access cone signals (Jacobs et al. 1996). With this technique, a flickering monochromatic test light (derived from a 15-nm bandpass monochromator supplied with either a 100 W tungsten-halide lamp or a 150 W xenon arc lamp) is continuously adjusted in intensity until the fundamental component of the ERG, it elicits is identical to that obtained in response to an interleaved, similarly flickering, reference light. The comparison of the responses to the test and reference lights is based on averages of the last 50 of a total of 70 responses elicited by each light. The reference light is derived from a 100 W tungsten-halide lamp with test and reference lights aligned so that they optically superimpose. A third stimulus beam, used to provide accessory adaptation, also originates from a 100 W tungsten-halide lamp and is optically imaged coincident with the other two beams. Further details of the stimuli used in these experiments are noted below as they become appropriate.

Results

Although ERGs could be readily recorded from the eye of the opossum, cone-based signals were quite small relative to those we have recorded from the eyes of a number of other strongly nocturnal mammals using similar techniques, e. g., two species of procyonids (Jacobs and Deegan 1992). Using a tungsten-halide lamp as the monochromator source, it proved possible to measure spectral sensitivity functions in 10 nm steps across the spectrum from 460 to 650 nm. For these measurements, the flicker rate was 20 Hz and the reference light was achromatic (2,850 K) yielding a retinal illuminance of 2.7 log td. All wavelengths were tested twice and the resulting photometric equation values were subsequently averaged. The averaged spectral sensitivity values (±1SD) so obtained for seven opossums (5 males, 2 females) are shown in Fig. 1. It is apparent that despite the small amplitudes of the cone signals the variability in spectral sensitivity across the sample is quite small. The full array of sensitivity values was best fit using a standard visual pigment template (Govardovskii et al. 2000) that was shifted along the wavelength axis in steps of 0.1 nm to find the spectral location that provided the best least squares fit to the full data array. A template (shown as the continuous line in Fig. 1) having λ max = 562.4 nm provides a good account of the averaged data. For similar fits made to the data arrays obtained from the seven animals individually, the mean λmax was 561.4 nm (SD = 1.78).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Spectral sensitivity of the Virginia opossum ERG measured for test conditions designed to capture cone signals. The solid circles are mean values for seven animals (±1 SD). These values were best fit to a visual pigment absorption function having a peak value of 562.4 nm (continuous line). Details of the test conditions and fitting procedure are given in the text

In an attempt to extend the spectral sensitivity measurements to encompass a greater spectral span a xenon lamp was substituted as the light source for the monochromator. All other details were as described above. The xenon light source allowed measurement of spectral sensitivity to be made in 10 nm steps from 370 to 670 nm. The results obtained from a single animal are shown in Fig. 2. As before, the resulting sensitivity measurements were best fit using a visual pigment template. The estimated peak is close to that for the more spectrally truncated function of Fig. 1—in this case having a λmax of 561.6 nm. Note that the template provides a reasonable account of the entire span of spectral sensitivity and that, in particular, there is no obvious elevation of sensitivity in the short wavelengths beyond that predicted by contribution from a single type of photopigment.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Extended cone spectral sensitivity of the ERG of the Virginia opossum. The solid circles are values obtained from a single animal. These have been best fit to a visual pigment absorption function having a peak value of 561.6 nm (continuous line). Other details are specified in the text

ERG spectral sensitivity functions recorded from opossums suggest the retina of this marsupial contains a cone pigment with a peak value of ~561–562 nm, but they give no obvious indication for the presence of any additional types of cone pigment. It is a standard assumption that if only a single pigment contributes to the spectral sensitivity then that spectral sensitivity curve should behave univariantly in the presence of chromatic adaptation (Rushton 1972). We conducted numerous experiments in an attempt to evaluate this possibility. In each of these cases, we measured some aspect of spectral sensitivity while the retina was concurrently chromatically adapted.

No indications of a failure of univariance were detected. Three separate examples that support this conclusion are shown in Fig. 3. In one case, a complete spectral sensitivity function (from 390 to 670 nm) was recorded using ERG flicker photometry under the conditions specified above. The function was then re-measured in the identical fashion except in the second instance the eye was concomitantly exposed to a long-wavelength light (produced by placing a long-pass filter having 50% transmission at 570 nm into the adaptation beam that produces a light appearing bright orange to a human). The effectiveness of this manipulation was established by determining the intensity of a 610 nm test light required to produce an ERG having a mean amplitude of 1.5 µV, first under neutral adaptation and then again in the presence of the orange adaptation light. So measured, this adaptation light produced a 0.9 log unit decrease in sensitivity. Figure 3a plots the wavelength-by-wavelength differences in sensitivity measured under the two test conditions as established by ERG flicker photometry. The variations in sensitivity are quite small (0.15 log units or less at all of the test wavelengths) and these variations show no systematic pattern across the spectrum, counter to what would be expected if more than one pigment contributed to the recorded ERG.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Results from three separate tests to assess whether the opossum cone ERG behaves univariantly in the face of concurrent chromatic adaptation. a Wavelength-by-wavelength differences (in log units) in spectral sensitivity obtained from measurements made in the neutrally adapted eye and then repeated as the eye was concurrently adapted to a bright 570 nm light. The best-fit regression (continuous line) indicates that there is no systematic change in spectral sensitivity between the two test conditions (r 2 = 0.0007, P > 0.05). b ERG flicker photometric equations obtained between a 560 nm reference light and two test lights (squares 440 nm, circles 390 nm). The equations were measured first in the absence of chromatic adaptation and then under increasing levels of chromatic adaptation (560 nm). The results are plotted as a function of the threshold elevation produced for the reference light and they show no systematic change with chromatic adaptation (440 nm, r 2 = 0.37, P = 0.31; 390 nm, r 2 = 0.54, P = 0.38). c Threshold elevations obtained for three test lights (560, 440, 390 nm) induced by increasing levels of concurrent, long-wavelength adaptation. The results for the latter two (squares and circles, respectively) are plotted as a function of the threshold elevation for the 560 nm light. The best-fit regressions have respective slopes of 0.98 (440 nm) and 1.05 (390 nm) with r 2 values for both = 0.98. Neither of the two are significantly different from a theoretical prediction of a slope of 1.0: 390 nm t(6) 7.365, P = 0.489; 440 nm, t(7) = 0.115, P = 0.911. Additional details for all three experiments are given in the text

A second type of experiment yielded a very similar outcome. In this case, ERG flicker photometric equations were made between a 560 nm reference light and two different test lights (440 and 390 nm). These test wavelengths were chosen as being near optimal for detecting the presence of contributions from cones containing either UV or S pigment. Equations were made first in the absence of any adaptation light and then repeatedly in the presence of an orange adaptation light (derived as above). The effectiveness of the adaptation was established by determining at each adaptation light level the intensity of a 560 nm light required to yield a signal having criterion amplitude of 1 µV. Figure 3b shows the result for one animal, but others behaved similarly. Across a range of adaptation light intensity up to a level sufficient to raise the threshold of the reference light by ~0.8 log units there is simply no change in the equation values, i.e., the relative sensitivity at the three wavelengths is unaffected by chromatic adaptation. In a third attempt, thresholds were measured for three test wavelengths (390, 440, and 560 nm) in the presence of progressively increasing levels of long-wavelength adaptation (produced as described above). The thresholds were defined using criterion amplitudes of 0.8 µV for the averaged response to 20 Hz flicker. The results are shown in Fig. 3c, where threshold elevations measured for the two shorter test lights (390 and 440 nm) are plotted against the corresponding threshold elevation recorded for the 560 nm test. Thresholds for the former two do not differ significantly from those recorded to the latter. In sum, in all three of these experiments, we found no changes in spectral sensitivity as would be predicted by a failure of univariance and thus no evidence that more than a single cone type contributes to the photopic ERG in this marsupial.

Discussion

The retina of the Viriginia opossum has long been known to have an abundant population of rods containing a typical rhodopsin (λ max = ~493 nm) (Crescitelli 1958), but there are apparently no previous measurements of cone pigments in this species. As noted, Hunt et al. have recently examined the opsin genes of two species of South American opossums, including one (D. domestica) that is a member of the same genus as the Virginia opossum (Hunt et al. 2009). In comparing the gene sequences for the LWS opsin gene, they isolated in the context of what had previously been learned about the identity of the amino acid residues associated with spectral tuning in other mammalian species they concluded that D. domestica would be predicted to have an LWS pigment with peak at about 555 nm. This inferred value is close to that we identified in the Virginia opossum. Whether the modest residual difference (555 vs. 561–562 nm) reflects a true species difference or might in some way be associated with differences that reflect some idiosyncrasies of the two estimation techniques is uncertain. In any case, the long-wavelength pigment inferred for the South American opossums is similar, if not identical, to that reflected in the electrophysiological recordings made on this North American species.

Two features of the opossum eye that might be thought to influence these spectral sensitivity measurements deserve brief mention. First is the possibility of some selective spectral filtering by the lens. There are no available measurements of lens transmission in this species but short-wavelength filtering by the lens would be expected to be modest in a strongly nocturnal species such as this one (Kennedy and Milkman 1956) and, in particular, would likely have not much or no effect on the functions of Fig. 1 that extend downward into the short wavelengths only to 460 nm. A second potential factor that might influence spectral sensitivity is the presence in the opossum eye of a retinal tapetum that covers a large fraction of the superior half of the eye giving this species a prominent eye shine. The reflecting material in the opossum tapetum consists of lipid spheres scattered in epithelial cells (Schwab et al. 2002; Ollivier et al. 2004). The reflective properties of this tapetum have not been measured, but to us the tapetal reflection from opossum eyes does not appear obviously colored. In an early examination, Gordon Walls described the opossum tapetum as being “slightly yellowish” in appearance (Walls 1939), but recent fundus photographs give no hint of any coloration (Schwab et al. 2002; Ollivier et al. 2004). If there is any influence of tapetal reflection on the spectral sensitivity of the opossum it is probably minimal.

Two observations made on the retina of the South American opossum D. auritai suggest that the retina of the Virginia opossum might likely be expected to contain a second, short-wavelength sensitive cone pigment. First, the monoclonal antibody OS-2 has been shown to selectively label short-wavelength sensitive cones in the retinas of a number of different mammalian species (Szel et al. 1996) and that marker was used to detect a small population of cones in D. aurita (Ahnelt et al. 1995). Second, the coding sequence for the SWS1 gene recently determined for this same species of opossum has been expressed and regenerated in vitro and absorbance measurements revealed that it was linked to a pigment with λ max of ~362 nm (Hunt et al. 2009). If one assumes that such a UV pigment may also be present in the Virginia opossum, our failure to detect its presence in ERG recordings could have a number of explanations. Perhaps the most likely one has to do with the sparsity of cones and their consequent feeble contribution to the ERG. Opsin immunolabeling indicates that the overall density of cones in D. aurita is not more than ~2,500 per mm2 while those believed to be short-wavelength sensitive do not exceed ~200 per mm2 (Ahnelt et al. 1995). By way of comparison, corresponding values for cone representation in the retina of a nocturnal rodent, the pigmented rat, an animal from which signals from both M and UV cones are easily detectable in the ERG are, respectively, ~7,000 per mm2 and 700–800 per mm2 (Jacobs et al. 2001; Ortin-Martiniz et al. 2010).

What functional roles might cones play in the vision of the opossum? An early behavioral experiment claimed to demonstrate the presence of color vision in the Virginia opossum (Friedman 1967). However, the lights among which color discriminations were tested in that experiment all lay in the visible part of the spectrum, a range over which we could detect the presence of only a single cone pigment and such an arrangement would obviate color vision. The rather coarse variation in stimulus intensity used in the early behavioral experiment suggests the alternative interpretation that the successful discriminations among the various colored lights may well have been based on the use of brightness cues. On examining the opossum retina, Walls suggested that, “the sparse cone populations cannot mean much to the animals” (Walls 1939). Whether these cones merely represent a vestigial hangover of the sauropsid past of this species, as Walls was led to conclude, or whether the cones continue to serve an adaptive function that is sufficient to maintain their presence could only be established by direct and careful behavioral examinations. At the least, our observations on the ERG in this species suggest that cone-based vision in this animal is apt to be considerably limited and, more broadly, that it is probably unwarranted to assume that the presence of two types of opsin genes and/or two classes of labeled cones in the nocturnal opossums necessarily implies the presence of a useful dichromatic color vision capacity.