Introduction

Reproductive timing and investment decisions exhibited by individuals of both sexes can be a strong determinant of fitness, and some reproductive strategies may outperform others (Lott 1991; Shapiro 1991). Reproductive success may be enhanced when individuals derive fitness benefits from specific environmental cues that indicate favourable conditions for reproduction (Gross 1996; Warner 1997; Brockman and Taborsky 2008). In marine systems, reproducing individuals commonly experience heterogeneous and dynamic environments that may vary greatly in their risks and rewards. Such species often respond to specific environmental cues that enable them to optimize their reproductive success. For example, many temperate fishes initiate reproduction in response to changes in water temperature, and this may lead to reproductive phenologies that improve food availability for their offspring (i.e. the ‘Match-Mismatch hypothesis’ of Cushing 1975). However, in tropical marine environments with less seasonal variation, many coral reef fishes spawn continuously throughout the year (exhibiting ‘extreme iteroparity’, Philippi and Seger 1989; Warner 1998; Wilbur and Rudolf 2006; Shima et al. 2018). Some researchers have equated the existence of extreme iteroparity in tropical reef fish species as an evolutionary response to an extremely unpredictable marine environment for offspring survival (e.g. ‘bet hedging strategy’, see Goodman, 1984; Einum and Fleming 2004; Hughes 2017). Others have hypothesized that adults may try to reduce the effect of density-dependent selection pressures on post-settlement offspring mortality, by spreading their reproductive effort through time (Shima et al. 2018). Alternatively, extreme iteroparity may simply be the best strategy when there is no clear advantage to spawning at one time or another.

Species that exhibit extreme iteroparity may be less reliant on external cues for reproductive investment decisions (Seger and Brockman 1987). Indeed, reproductive investment by many iteroparous, pelagic spawners appears to be relatively invariant in time and space (Warner 1995; Sancho et al. 2000; Claydon et al. 2014). Spawning aggregations of many of these species form on a daily basis and often during a particular time of day (Robertson 1983; Domeier and Colin 1997; Sancho et al. 2000; Claydon et al. 2014). Such spawning activities also commonly occur in particular locations, typically associated with geomorphological features such as the down-current edges of reef slopes (Randall and Randall 1963; Colin and Bell 1991; Warner 1995; Heyman and Kjerfve 2008; Claydon et al. 2014).

Many protogynous reef fishes have haremic social structures and may spawn in pairs and/or smaller groups (Warner and Hoffman 1980a, 1980b; Warner 1984; Kuwamura et al. 2009). Although reproduction is spread along a temporal continuum, spawning intensity may still vary through space and time (Johannes 1978; Robertson 1983; Petersen et al. 1992; Gust 2004; Claydon et al. 2014). For such species, the number of individuals at a spawning site can vary from several spawning pairs to hundreds of individuals over the course of a few days (Warner and Hoffman 1980a, 1980b; Warner 1995; Domeier and Colin 1997; Claydon et al. 2014). Spawning frequencies can also vary substantially between sites that are separated by only a few metres (Warner and Hoffman 1980a, 1980b; Warner 1984; Petersen et al. 1992). Consequently, even extremely iteroparous species may use environmental cues to make informed decisions about reproductive allocation in order to maximize their fitness.

A variety of hypotheses have been proposed to explain spatio-temporal variation in reproductive allocation of highly iteroparous reef fish. For example, fluctuations in spawning might be a plastic response to specific oceanographic conditions that promote survival of eggs and larvae through to settlement (Johannes 1978; Lobel 1978; Robertson et al. 1990; Claydon 2004). Such species may choose to spawn at times or places where local currents facilitate fertilization success, and/or rapid transport fertilized eggs away from reef-associated benthic and pelagic predators (Johannes 1978; Lobel 1978; Sancho et al. 2000). In apparent support of this hypothesis, studies have documented a positive correlation between spawning intensity and tidal-driven, outflowing currents, and/or increased current speeds that may facilitate a rapid dispersal of propagules into oceanic waters (Sancho et al. 2000; Claydon et al. 2014; Donahue et al. 2015).

Additionally, reproductive decisions may be influenced by risks associated with spawning (Warner 1998). Highly iteroparous individuals should normally prioritize their own survival over the slight fitness increases that may be gained from any individual reproductive bout, because these benefits may be greatly outweighed by the fitness gains from future reproduction. Consequently, highly iteroparous species may be more risk averse and should only intensify their spawning activities when conditions enhance their own survival, and conversely, they should avoid spawning in times or places of high predation risk. Environmental conditions that increase predation risk (e.g. low light levels, reduced visibility, or extremely exposed spawning territories) should be avoided (Robertson and Hoffman 1977; Shapiro et al. 1988; Warner 1998; Claydon et al. 2014).

However, because the consequences of local conditions on fitness may be difficult to predict (as they are too variable in time and space), spawning periodicity in iteroparous species may reflect a generalized adaptive response to an easily detectable, larger-scale environmental cue (Warner 1997). Environmental cues that prevail over a broad geographic range may not necessarily always maximize fitness; however, they may guarantee the highest average fitness for a population over a reproductive season. Many studies suggest that periodic spawning activities (across a wide range of taxa) are correlated with the lunar cycle—potentially because the lunar cycle represents such an easily observable, large-scale cue (Robertson 1983; Robertson et al. 1990; Zeller 1998; Claydon et al. 2014; Shima et al. 2020). The lunar state could simply serve as a convenient cue to facilitate synchronized reproduction (Lobel 1978; Colin and Clavijo 1988; Colin & Bell 1991; Claydon 2004) or to couple reproduction with the optimal timing of settlement (Robertson et al. 1988; Doherty et al. 1994; Shima et al. 2018). Additionally, spawning at particular portions of the lunar cycle may enhance dispersal of fertilized eggs (e.g. via tidal effects: Robertson et al. 1990; Claydon 2004; Claydon et al. 2014), and/or growth and survival of eggs and larvae in the offshore habitat (e.g. via effects related to moonlight: Shima and Swearer 2019; Shima et al 2020).

Here, we evaluate temporal and spatial variation in spawning patterns of the sixbar wrasse, Thalassoma hardwicke, in relation to broad and local scale environmental conditions. The sixbar wrasse is a small-bodied, protogynous reef fish. Some level of spawning is observable most days throughout the year and primarily concentrated during a 2-h period in the afternoon (Mitterwallner 2020). Socially dominant males establish mating territories at particular locations on a daily basis. We took advantage of the predictability of this system to address the following questions: (1) How does spawning intensity vary among days? (2) What are the potential environmental drivers of reproductive investment decisions? (3) To what extent are reproductive patterns associated with local cues (and their interactions), and to what extent are they influenced by larger-scale environmental cues like the lunar phase?

Methods

Study site and species

We surveyed spawning patterns of the sixbar wrasse (Thalassoma hardwicke; henceforth, sixbars) from February to June 2017, at two focal sites on the north shore of Moorea, French Polynesia. Sixbar wrasse are widely distributed throughout the Indo-Pacific region, and they are one of the most conspicuous and abundant reef fishes within inshore reef habitats of French Polynesia (Galzin 1987). Sixbar wrasse are protogynous hermaphrodites (Warner 1984a). Local populations contain a mix of initial phase (IP) females and males (which are morphologically indistinguishable from one another) and terminal-phase (TP) males (which are distinguishable by their brighter colour patterns and blunter heads). Sixbars are highly iteroparous, and they spawn throughout the lunar cycle and across much of the year (Claydon et al. 2014). Like other members of the genus (e.g. Thalassoma bifasciatum; Warner 1984a; Warner 1995), sixbars migrate to particular areas (e.g. reef edges) to spawn, with the largest females travelling as far as 400 m to reach spawning areas (Mitterwallner 2020). At these locations, TP males defend mating territories and actively court females. Spawners may engage in both pair spawning (i.e. one TP male and one female) and group spawning. IP males frequently attempt to ‘sneak’ mating opportunities during bouts of pair spawning, and they appear to participate openly in group spawning. On Moorea, most sixbar spawning activity is concentrated from 1300 to 1600 h daily, and consistently maximal from ~ 1400–1500 (P. Mitterwallner unpublished data).

Moorea is surrounded by a barrier reef crest that delineates the outermost extent of the lagoon system (spanning a width of ~ 1 km from reef crest to shore). Much of the lagoon is dominated by wide, flat areas (1–5 m depth) of sand and rubble, interspersed with patches of coral, algal turf, and extensive stands of macroalgae (Galzin and Pointier 1985). Water is forced into the lagoon by wave action and generally flows shoreward from the reef crest and then back out to sea through breaks (i.e. ‘passes’) in the reef crest (Galzin and Pointier 1985). Many species including sixbars spawn at the reef edge near these passes (P. Mitterwallner unpublished data).

Surveys of spawning activity

We conducted preliminary surveys to determine focal locations, durations, and times of day for subsequent spawning observations. Based on these initial surveys, we selected two focal sites (hereafter, S1 and S2) situated at the reef edge, near passes, on the north-eastern side of the island (see Fig. 1). Within each site, we identified and mapped three focal territories (T1, T2, T3) of terminal-phase males. These territories were distributed along a gradient from exposed (i.e. T1 closest to the reef edge) to sheltered conditions (i.e. T3 furthest from the reef edge; Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

The two surveyed spawning sites (S1) and (S2) within a lagoon at the north shore of Moorea, French Polynesia (a, b). Within each site, we monitored focal territories (T1, T2, T3) of terminal-phase males (c). These territories were distributed along a gradient from exposed (i.e. T1 closest to the reef edge) to sheltered conditions (i.e. T3 furthest from the reef edge)

We surveyed focal territories in each site on alternate days. Each site was surveyed between 2 and 4 pm, ~ 3–5 times per week over 5 lunar cycles (i.e. 20 weeks). During each survey, a single observer (PM) recorded spawning activities within each focal TP territory (n = 3) over a 20-min duration. At each survey, focal territories were observed sequentially (i.e. over ~ 1 h), in randomized order, and during peak spawning activity. ‘Spawning’ was evidenced by female(s) and focal male(s) rising towards the surface and releasing eggs and sperm simultaneously. We estimated spawning intensity as the cumulative number of spawning events (i.e. pooling all pair spawns, group spawns, sneaking and streaking events). At the start of each survey, we estimated densities of fish at the spawning site by quantifying IP and TP fish along a single, permanent transect (100 m × 5 m width) centred on the spawning site. We surveyed this transect three times on each day of observation. We also recorded a set of 7 environmental variables during each observation period (detailed in Table 1).

Table 1 Measured environmental variables that potentially affect spawning activities. Given are the methods of estimation for each variable and a qualitative description of the resulting data

Data analysis

We used a periodic regression (linear-circular regression) approach to model the cyclical nature of the lunar cycle and to evaluate primary sources of variation in sixbar spawning activity over time. Following methods of deBruyn and Meeuwig (2001), the calendar date of each observation was converted to a ‘lunar day’ (0–29.5; day 0 corresponds to the new moon, day 15 to the full moon, etc.). Lunar days were then divided into 360° (or 2π radians) to allocate each day an angular equivalent, theta (θ). The cyclical pattern of the lunar calendar was then expressed by sine and cosine transformations of theta. The cosine term describes a phase shift near 0° or 180° (full moon and new moon), and the sine term describes a phase shift between 90 and 270° (first and last quarter). A positive cosθ coefficient represents a peak at new moon, and a negative cosθ coefficient a peak at full moon. A significant positive sinθ coefficient corresponds to a peak during the first quarter, and a significant negative sin coefficient corresponds to a peak during the last quarter. The transformed data can then be analysed by using simple linear or linear mixed effect regression.

We used general linear mixed effect models (glmmTMB package, Brooks et al. 2017; R Core Team 2019) with a Poisson error distribution (because our observations consisted of counts) to evaluate the main sources of variation in (1) sixbar spawning activity and (2) population densities at the spawning site. Initially, we fitted a fully saturated model with all explanatory variables. The fully saturated model included the explanatory variables as fixed effects: ‘site’ (e.g. S1 or S2), ‘territory location (T1, T2, or T3), ‘sinθ, cosθ, ‘temperature’, ‘daytime cloud cover’, ‘night-time cloud cover’ (previous night), ‘visibility’, ‘current speed’, and ‘current direction’ (see Table 1 and Table 1A for details). The full model also included ‘Date’ (coded numerically) as a random effect. For spawning activities, we also included all possible two-way interactions between the local environmental variables ‘current speed’, ‘current direction’, ‘visibility’, and ‘cloud cover’ to evaluate whether co-occurrence of specific conditions might disproportionally influence spawning activities (e.g. do spawning frequencies significantly increase if outflowing currents co-occur with elevated current speeds?).

To account for potential temporal autocorrelation, we incorporated an AR(1) covariance structure in the model, as described in the glmmTMB vignette (Brooks et al. 2017). The covariance structure in the models was represented by ‘territory id’ as the random intercept and ‘date’ as grouping parameter to account for temporal autocorrelation between territories. This significantly improved the fit of the initial model based on p-value, AICc comparisons, and DHARMA (Hartig 2018) residual plots, suggesting that results were serially autocorrelated. To validate model suitability, we checked for uniformity of the residual quantiles (observed vs expected), as well as for overdispersion and outliers in the diagnostic plots, using the DHARMA package in R (Hartig 2018). In all cases the fits of the global models appeared satisfactory. We then used Akaike’s information criterion corrected for small sample sizes (AICc) to identify an appropriately reduced model (i.e. the candidate model with the lowest AICc; obtained via multimodel selection methods developed by the MuMIn package in R, see Bartón 2015). If the top set of models only varied slightly in their data fit, we performed a model averaging approach across all models with < 2 ΔAICc (the difference between a given model and the model with the lowest AICc), to generate more robust parameter estimates (Burnham and Anderson 2002). Model averaging was employed with the model.avg function of the MuMIn package in R (Bartón 2015). This function calculates a weighted average of each predictor variable appearing in the top candidate model set (i.e. all candidate models with ΔAICc < 2). Additionally, we calculated 95% confidence intervals for each predictor in the final averaged model, to determine whether an explanatory variable had a significant effect on the response variable (predictors with confidence intervals that overlap zero are usually not significant). The relative importance of a given predictor was then quantified as the sum of Akaike weights of each model (ΔAICc < 2) in which a particular predictor is represented (RVI > 0.9 = high importance; RVI > 0.6 = moderate importance; RVI < 0.6 = low importance; RVI < 0.5 = no importance, see Burnham and Anderson 2002).

To visualize the predicted effects of significant factors on the dependent variables, we additionally calculated estimated marginal means for each predictor, with covariates held at their mean value (function emmeans in the R package emmeans, see Lenth 2018).

Results

In total, we observed 649 bouts of spawning over a period of 150 days. On average, we recorded 3.2 spawning events per 20-min observation period, and spawning frequency varied substantially through time (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Average number of spawns per day (averaged over three territories) over 5 lunar cycles from February to June. Black circles indicate new moon phases; white circles indicate full moon phases. Error bars represent ± 1SE

Sources of variation in overall spawning frequency

Model selection yielded 15 models with a delta AICc < 2 for overall spawning frequencies as response variable (i.e. the total number of spawning events observed per 20-min observation period). The averaged model (i.e. the average over all models AICc < 2) retained moon phase (sinθ as well as the cosθ term), territory position, an interaction between cloud cover and current speed, current direction, visibility, and location as explanatory variables (see Table 2 and attachment Table A1).

Table 2 Model averaged parameter estimates and test statistics explaining total spawning frequency of Thalassoma hardwicke at spawning sites (averaged over all models with a delta AICc < 2). Confidence intervals that do not contain zero (i.e. indicate statistical significance) are indicated in bold text. Relative variable importance (RVI) reflects how many models with a delta AICc < 2 included each explanatory variable, while accounting for the weight of these models. An RVI of > 0.9 indicates high importance, an RVI > 0.6 = moderate importance and an RVI < 0.5 indicates no significant effect

However, confidence intervals as well as relative variable importance suggested that only moon phase and territory location (i.e. proximity to the reef edge) had a significant effect on overall spawning frequency (Fig. 3a, b and Table 2). A positive cosθ regression coefficient in the final model indicated significantly higher spawning activities around the new moon in comparison with full moon periods (0.867 ± 0.168 SE; 95% CI: 0.535, 1.198, RVI = 1.0). A marginally non-significant negative sinθ regression coefficient with an RVI of 0.8 suggested elevated activities of spawning during the third quarter (-0.266 ± 0.203 SE; 95% CI: -0.666, 0.134, RVI = 0.8). Together, these results suggest a steady increase of spawning activities from first quarter/full moon towards third quarter/new moon, with greatest activity peaks around the new moon period (roughly 6 times higher at new moon relative to full moon; Fig. 3a). Spawning intensity was also strongly influenced by territory position: exposed territories close to the reef edge exhibited significantly higher spawning frequencies relative to more sheltered territories (territory 3: -2.049 ± 0.231 SE; 95% CI: -2.505, -1.592, RVI = 1.0, 30–50% decrease in T3 and T2 relative to T1, Fig. 3b).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Sources of variation in overall sixbar spawning frequency through time. a Smoothed curve (± 95% confidence interval) of the lunar effect on predicted spawning frequency during the 29.5-day lunar cycle). b Variation in spawning among territories (T1 is closest to reef edge, T3 furthest from reef edge). Panels b gives marginal means back-transformed to the original scale (± SE) estimated with other fixed effects held at their mean values

Model averaging revealed that the local variables that were retained in the final model (i.e. interaction between cloud cover and current speed, visibility, current direction, and spawning location) had 95% confidence intervals that included zero, and a relative variable importance (RVI) of < 0.5 (see Table 2). This strongly suggests that local physical variables did not have a significant effect on variation of spawning frequencies at the spawning sites.

Population densities at the spawning site

Densities of sixbars at the spawning site ranged from 7.5 to 66 individuals/500 m2 per observation period. Linear regression analysis revealed that spawning activities were significantly positively correlated with population densities at the spawning site (F1,38 = 5.87, p = 0.02, Fig. 4a). Model averaging suggested that densities of sixbars that aggregated at the spawning sites were most strongly associated with the cosθ term of the final model, with highest population densities at new moon (0.317 ± 0.099 SE; 95% CI: 0.117, 0.517, RVI = 1.0; see Table 3 and attachment Table A2, Fig. 4b). Additionally, ‘spawning location’ was found to be an important term after model averaging, with higher population densities at spawning site S2 (0.467 ± 0.146 SE; 95% CI: 0.170, 0.763, RVI = 1.0; see Table 3). Apart from the ‘cosθ’ term and ‘spawning site’, ‘current direction’ and ‘sinθ’ remained in the final model. However, confidence intervals as well as RVI indicated that neither current direction nor sinθ proved to be important predictors of changes in population densities at the spawning sites (see Table 3).

Fig. 4
figure 4

a Smoothed curve (± 95% confidence interval) of the lunar effect on predicted spawning frequency during the 29.5-day lunar cycle. b Linear regression between population density at the local spawning site and number of spawns per 20 min. P-values and regression coefficient indicate a significant positive relationship

Table 3 Model selection table of all the models with a delta AICc < 2 explaining variation in population densities of Thalassoma hardwicke at spawning sites during spawning hours. The fully saturated model contained cloud cover night-time (CC.n), cloud cover day-time (CC.d), cosθ, current direction (CD), current speed (CS), spawning location (SL), sinθ, territory (T), visibility (V), and date (random factor). All models were fit using the R package glmmTMB (Poisson distribution)

Discussion

Sixbar wrasse spawned on nearly every day of our 150d observation period, but the intensity of spawning varied strongly with the lunar cycle. Spawning was maximal around the new moon and greatest at male territories closest to the reef edge. Other abiotic variables were uncorrelated or weakly correlated with spawning intensity.

Lunar periodicity

Potential drivers of lunar periodicity: offspring survival

Many reef fishes exhibit lunar periodicity in spawning activities, with spawning particularly common around the new and/or full moon (Johannes 1978; Takemura et al. 2004; Claydon et al. 2014). In situations where the environments of offspring may be rapidly changing or difficult to predict, a generalized response to an easily detectable large-scale cue like the lunar state may provide several selective advantages for spawners (Warner 1997). For sixbars, spawning close to the new moon may maximize fitness by enhancing survival of adults and/or growth and survival of offspring.

Temporal patterns of spawning are frequently linked to hydrodynamic regimes that facilitate offshore transport of eggs and larvae (Johannes 1978; Sancho et al. 2000; Claydon et al. 2014). However, local current dynamics in shallow lagoon habitats may be a poor predictor for dispersal processes because local wind conditions and shear-flow generated eddies can generate substantial variability in current patterns around the spawning site (see Warner 1997; Ezer et al. 2011). This may explain why our analyses failed to detect any strong relationships between spawning activity and local current velocity/direction. Maximum tidal amplitudes during new and full moon phases that promote a persistent, large-scale offshore transport despite the local variability, may provide spawners with a more useful (generalized) cue for transport-related processes (Robertson et al. 1990; Warner 1997; Claydon 2004; Claydon et al. 2014). However, if sixbars are only attempting to spawn during peak tidal amplitudes, then they might be expected to exhibit semi-lunar spawning patterns (i.e. spawning disproportionately on both the new moon and full moon, when tidal amplitudes are similarly maximal). So, at best, lunar-driven offshore dispersal provides only a partial explanation for the patterns of spawning that we observed. Alternatively, we note that our sampling of reproductive activities may have been insufficient to detect more subtle patterns of variation, and/or correlations with some environmental variables.

Lunar illumination might influence the fitness of dispersing eggs and larvae. Many juvenile reef fish settle back to the reef at night (Dufour and Galzin 1993) and during new moon, presumably to reduce their risk to predators (Sponaugle and Cowen 1994; Robertson et al. 1999; Shima and Swearer 2019). Similarly, the degree of lunar illumination at night might drive reproductive decisions of adults. Since a majority of iteroparous, protogynous species spawn in the afternoon (Colin and Clavijo 1988; Sancho et al. 2000; Claydon et al. 2014), dark nights during the new moon might reduce predation pressure on recently fertilized eggs that are still in close proximity to the reef.

If the developmental duration of pelagic larvae is invariant, the precise timing of spawning might dictate the timing of larval settlement to the benthos. Reef fish might use the lunar phase as an environmental cue to spawn at times of the month that facilitate their offspring’s settlement at a favourable time (e.g. the new moon; Robertson et al. 1988; Shima et al. 2018). Coral trout (Plectropomus leopardus) appear to exemplify this: spawning occurs around the new moon (Samoilys 1997), larvae develop for ~ 25 days in the pelagic (Doherty et al. 1994), and then settle back to the reef close to the following new moon (when risk from nocturnally active predators is presumed to be lowest). Temporal coupling of production and settlement has also been observed for the damselfish Stegastes partitus in the Caribbean (Robertson et al., 1988). Sixbars also spawn and settle disproportionately on the new moon (Shima et al. 2018). However, they have an average pelagic larval duration (PLD) of 47 days (Victor 1986; Shima et al. 2020). Given the preponderance of spawning close to the new moon, a PLD of 47 days would seem to indicate that settlement should commonly occur close to the full moon (a pattern that is not observed; Shima et al. 2018). This apparent conundrum is reconciled, at least in part, by plasticity in larval development time: PLD is variable in this species (range: 37–61 days), and larvae (that survive to successfully settle) appear to adjust their PLD to settle closer to the new moon irrespective of their date of spawning (Shima et al., 2020). If adaptive plasticity in PLD effectively decouples spawning dates from settlement dates for sixbars, this suggests that their lunar periodicity in spawning might not be related to selection pressures associated with settlement. So why then do they spawn disproportionately around the new moon? One possible explanation is that spawning on a new moon results in a growth advantage to larvae that survive to settlement (Shima et al 2021). The mechanisms underlying this pattern remain untested, but may be related to interactions with a community of organisms that migrate vertically in the water column (see Shima et al 2021, 2022 for the full hypothesis). The growth advantage associated with spawning on the new moon may improve fitness of subsequent life stages, because fish that settle at a larger size may have a future competitive advantage (Forrester 1991; Sogard 1997; Shima and Findlay 2002). Newly settled sixbars appear to have a size-based dominance hierarchy (personal observations), and individuals that settle larger may have better access to limited food and shelter resources (Forrester 1991; Shima and Findlay 2002; McCormick & Hoey 2004). If these size-related advantages persist and compound through time, then offspring that are born around the new moon (and survive to settle) may ultimately be more likely survive to sex change (and transform into territorial males) due to their size-related competitive abilities. In a protogynous breeding system, territorial males achieve the highest levels of fitness because they can monopolize mating opportunities with many females (Warner and Hoffman 1980a, 1980b; Warner 1984a; Walker et al. 2007). Consequently, parents might periodically increase their reproductive effort around the new moon as a strategy to produce high-quality phenotypes (e.g. fast growers) that can maximize offspring production.

Potential drivers of lunar periodicity: adult survival

Lunar-cyclic reproduction may be an adaptive response to specific predator–prey dynamics that periodically increase mortality risk for sixbars adults at the spawning sites. We regularly observed spawning aggregations of common prey species like Chlorurus sordidus and Acanthurus nigrofuscus during full moon phases at the spawning sites. Since these small-bodied species usually reproduce in large, conspicuous aggregations near the reef edge, they might attract predators that hunt along the reef edges. For sixbar wrasse, concentrating reproductive activities around the third quarter/new moon may be a strategy to evade predators (i.e. reduce ‘apparent competition’) around the full moon (Shima et al., 2020).

Alternatively, particular phases of the moon might not provide any selective advantage, and the lunar cycle may simply be a timing cue to synchronize reproduction across individuals (Colin and Bell 1991; Domeier and Colin 1997; Claydon et al. 2014). We regularly observed smaller initial phase males and females forming group spawning aggregations within male territories during new moon. Higher densities of initial phase males (and corresponding group spawning activities) might simply be a response to an increased number of females at the mating site. However, synchrony of reproductive activities around the new moon might also represent a targeted strategy to improve survival chances of smaller individuals (that are particularly vulnerable to predators): group spawning may reduce per capita predation risk and/or improve predator detection due to an increased collective vigilance (see ‘many eyes hypothesis’, Roberts 1996). Additionally, constraining the portion of the month over which spawning occurs may enable smaller individuals to allocate more time to feeding (and growth) instead of to costly (and sometimes life-threatening) reproductive behaviours like sneaking, courtship, and migration (Hoffman 1983; Colin and Clavijo 1988; Molloy et al. 2012

Local environmental cues

Although our findings suggest that spawning intensity is most strongly shaped by a large-scale environmental cue (i.e. the lunar cycle), model predictions also indicate an important (albeit weaker) effect of local environmental conditions. Spawning intensity varied strongly with proximity to the reef edge. Male territories closest to the reef edge in our study had ~ sixfold more spawning events than those furthest from the reef edge. This effect size is similar to the magnitude of increase in spawning intensity from full moon to new moon. Despite intense courtship efforts of territorial males in sheltered territories (i.e. furthest from the reef edge), most females (particularly the largest ones) travelled through those territories to mate with males that occupied the more exposed territories (i.e. closest to the reef edge). Potentially, exposed territories might provide an advantage immediately following the release of eggs (Hensley et al. 1994): shear flows along outer reef edges produce hydrological turbulences that may rapidly entrain the fertilized eggs into larger current systems, transporting them beyond the reach of potential egg predators (Colin and Clavijo 1988; Claydon 2004; Hamner and Largier 2011). When spawning did occur in sheltered territories, it tended to involve smaller individuals (Mitterwallner 2020). This suggests some risk-based decision making by spawners. Hence, territory selection might be mediated by a site-specific trade-off between predation pressure on adults, and the initial survival potential of released eggs (Robertson and Hoffman 1977; Sancho et al. 2000; Claydon 2004).

Our study incorporated a wide range of environmental variables. This enabled us to evaluate potential spawning cues (and their interactions) that operate across a range of scales. Our findings suggest that temporal variation in spawning intensity of the sixbar wrasse is overwhelmingly shaped by a large-scale, global cue (the lunar cycle). In a temporarily variable environment, purely conditional adjustments may be evolutionary detrimental, since individuals are not able to predict long-term consequences on offspring dispersal (Lloyd 1984; Moran 1992; Warner 1997). Consequently, selection may favour fixed responses to a global cue (the lunar cycle), as a mechanism that guarantees intermediate offspring fitness across a range of environmental states (Moran 1992; Warner 1997). Additionally, we hypothesize that lunar-cyclic reproduction might represent a strategy to maximize adult survival potential. Our study also suggests that parents seem to utilize a detectable local cue for reproduction: territory location appears to convey sufficient information in terms of initial offspring survival to elicit behavioural responses over short timescales.