Arnold K, Zuberbühler K (2006) The alarm-calling system of adult male putty-nosed monkeys, Cercopithecus nictitans martini. Anim Behav 72:643–653. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.11.017
Article
Google Scholar
Baker MC, Becker AM (2002) Mobbing calls of black-capped chickadees: effects of urgency on call production. Wilson Bull 114:510–516
Article
Google Scholar
Berthet M, Mesbahi G, Pajot A, Cäsar C, Neumann C, Zuberbühler K (2019) Titi monkeys combine alarm calls to create probabilistic meaning. Sci Adv 5:eaav3991. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aav3991
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Berthet M, Neumann C, Mesbahi G, Cäsar C, Zuberbühler K (2018) Contextual encoding in titi monkey alarm call sequences. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 72:8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-017-2424-z
Article
Google Scholar
Boesch C (1994) Cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees. Anim Behav 48:653–667. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1994.1285
Article
Google Scholar
Boesch C, Boesch H (1989) Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. Am J Phys Anthropol 78:547–573. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.1330780410
CAS
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Buzzard PJ (2010) Polyspecific associations of Cercopithecus campbelli and C. petaurista with C. diana: what are the costs and benefits? Primates 51:307–314. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-010-0203-9
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Buzzard PJ (2006) Ecological partitioning of Cercopithecus campbelli, C. petaurista, and C. diana in the Taï Forest. Int J Primatol 27:529–558. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-006-9022-7
Article
Google Scholar
Candiotti A, Zuberbühler K, Lemasson A (2012) Convergence and divergence in Diana monkey vocalizations. Biol Lett 8:382–385. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2011.1182
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Caro TM (1986) The functions of stotting: a review of the hypotheses. Anim Behav 34:649–662. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80051-3
Article
Google Scholar
Casamitjana J (2002) The vocal repertoire of the woolly monkey Lagothrix lagothricha. Bioacoustics 13:1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2002.9753483
Article
Google Scholar
Cäsar C, Byrne RW, Hoppitt W, Young RJ, Zuberbühler K (2012) Evidence for semantic communication in titi monkey alarm calls. Anim Behav 84:405–411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.05.010
Article
Google Scholar
Chapman CA, Chapman LJ (1996) Mixed-species primate groups in the kibale forest: ecological constraints on association. Int J Primatol 17:31–50. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02696157
Article
Google Scholar
Chapman CA, Chapman LJ (2000) Interdemic variation in mixed-species association patterns: common diurnal primates of Kibale National Park, Uganda. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 47:129–139. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650050003
Article
Google Scholar
Chung Y, Rabe-Hesketh S, Dorie V, Gelman A, Liu J (2013) A nondegenerate penalized likelihood estimator for variance parameters in multilevel models. Psychometrika 78:685–709. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-013-9328-2
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Collier K, Radford AN, Stoll S, Watson SK, Manser MB, Bickel B, Townsend SW (2020) Dwarf mongoose alarm calls: investigating a complex non-human animal call. Proc R Soc B 287:20192514. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.2514
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cords M (1990) Vigilance and mixed-species association of some East African forest monkeys. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 26:297–300. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00178323
Article
Google Scholar
Covey R, McGraw WS (2014) Monkeys in a West African bushmeat market: implications for Cercopithecid conservation in Eastern Liberia. Trop Conserv Sci 7:115–125. https://doi.org/10.1177/194008291400700103
Article
Google Scholar
Coye C, Ouattara K, Zuberbühler K, Lemasson A (2015) Suffixation influences receivers’ behaviour in non-human primates. Proc R Soc B 282:20150265. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.0265
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Cunningham S, Magrath RD (2017) Functionally referential alarm calls in noisy miners communicate about predator behaviour. Anim Behav 129:171–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.05.021
Article
Google Scholar
Dawson Pell FSE, Potvin DA, Ratnayake CP, Fernández-Juricic E, Magrath RD, Radford AN (2018) Birds orient their heads appropriately in response to functionally referential alarm calls of heterospecifics. Anim Behav 140:109–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.04.010
Article
Google Scholar
Dezecache G, Berthet M (2018) Working hypotheses on the meaning of general alarm calls. Anim Behav 142:113–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.06.008
Article
Google Scholar
Eckardt W, Zuberbühler K (2004) Cooperation and competition in two forest monkeys. Behav Ecol 15:400–411. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arh032
Article
Google Scholar
Engesser S, Ridley AR, Townsend SW (2016) Meaningful call combinations and compositional processing in the southern pied babbler. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113:5976–5981. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1600970113
CAS
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Fallow PM, Magrath RD (2010) Eavesdropping on other species: mutual interspecific understanding of urgency information in avian alarm calls. Anim Behav 79:411–417. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.018
Article
Google Scholar
Fichtel C (2008) Ontogeny of conspecific and heterospecific alarm call recognition in wild Verreaux’s sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi). Am J Primatol 70:127–135. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20464
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2002) Anti-predator behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed evidence for a referential alarm call system. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:262–275. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-001-0436-0
Article
Google Scholar
Fichtel C, van Schaik CP (2006) Semantic differences in Sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi) alarm calls: a reflection of genetic or cultural variants? Ethology 112:839–849. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01239.x
Article
Google Scholar
Fischer J, Hammerschmidt K, Todt D (2010) Factors affecting acoustic variation in Barbary macaque (Macaca sylvanus) disturbance calls. Ethology 101:51–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1995.tb00345.x
Article
Google Scholar
Fischer J, Metz M, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2001) Baboon responses to graded bark variants. Anim Behav 61:925–931. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1687
Article
Google Scholar
Flower TP, Gribble M, Ridley AR (2014) Deception by flexible alarm mimicry in an African bird. Science 344:513. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1249723
CAS
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Foerster S (2008) Two incidents of venomous snakebite on juvenile blue and Sykes monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni and C. m. albogularis). Primates 49:300–303. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-008-0098-x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fox J, Weisberg S (2019) An {R} Companion to Applied Regression, version 3.0–10, https://socialsciences.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion/
Gamer M, Lemon J, Ian Fellows Puspendra Singh (2019) irr: Various Coefficients of Interrater Reliability and Agreement, R package version 0.84.1, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=irr
Gautier J-P, Gautier A (1977) Communication in old world monkeys. In: University I (ed) How animals communicate Sebeok TA. Press, Bloomington, pp 890–964
Google Scholar
Goodale E, Beauchamp G, Magrath RD, Nieh JC, Ruxton GD (2010) Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. Trends Ecol Evol 25:354–361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2010.01.002
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Goodale E, Ruxton GD, Beauchamp G (2019) Predator eavesdropping in a mixed-species environment: how prey species may use grouping, confusion, and the cocktail party effect to reduce predator detection. Front Ecol Evol 7:141. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2019.00141
Article
Google Scholar
Goodale E, Sridhar H, Sieving KE et al (2020) Mixed company: a framework for understanding the composition and organization of mixed-species animal groups. Biol Rev 95:889–910. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12591
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Graw B, Manser MB (2007) The function of mobbing in cooperative meerkats. Anim Behav 74:507–517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.11.021
Article
Google Scholar
Griesser M (2008) Referential calls signal predator behavior in a group-living bird species. Curr Biol 18:69–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.069
CAS
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Griesser M (2009) Mobbing calls signal predator category in a kin group-living bird species. Proc R Soc Lond B 276:2887–2892. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.0551
Article
Google Scholar
Grieves LA, Logue DM, Quinn JS (2014) Joint-nesting smooth-billed anis, Crotophaga ani, use a functionally referential alarm call system. Anim Behav 89:215–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.01.008
Article
Google Scholar
Heymann EW (2011) Coordination in primate mixed-species groups. In: Boos M, Kolbe M, Kappeler PM, Ellwart T (eds) Coordination in Human and Primate Groups. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 263–281
Chapter
Google Scholar
Hothorn T, Bretz F, Westfall P (2008) Simultaneous inference in general parametric. Biometrical J 50:346–363
Article
Google Scholar
Ito R, Mori A (2010) Vigilance against predators induced by eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls in a non-vocal lizard Oplurus cuvieri cuvieri (Reptilia: Iguania). Proc R Soc Lond B 277:1275–1280. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.2047
Article
Google Scholar
Jenny D (2005) Hunting behaviour in West African forest leopards. Afr J Ecol 43:197–200. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2028.2005.00565.x
Article
Google Scholar
Kane EE, McGraw WS (2017) Dietary variation in Diana monkeys (Cercopithecus diana): the effects of polyspecific associations. Folia Primatol 88:455–482. https://doi.org/10.1159/000484560
Article
Google Scholar
Keen SC, Cole EF, Sheehan MJ, Sheldon BC (2020) Social learning of acoustic anti-predator cues occurs between wild bird species. Proc R Soc B 287:20192513. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.2513
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Keenan S, Mathevon N, Stevens JM, Nicolè F, Zuberbühler K, Guéry JP, Levréro F (2020) The reliability of individual vocal signature varies across the bonobo’s graded repertoire. Anim Behav 169:9–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.08.024
Article
Google Scholar
Kershenbaum A, Blumstein DT, Roch MA et al (2016) Acoustic sequences in non-human animals: a tutorial review and prospectus. Biol Rev 91:13–52. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12160
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kirchhof J, Hammerschmidt K (2006) Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax) - evidence from playback experiments. Ethology 112:346–354. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01165.x
Article
Google Scholar
Klump GM, Kretzschmar E, Curio E (1986) The hearing of an avian predator and its avian prey. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:317–323. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00299662
Article
Google Scholar
Koné I, McGraw S, Gonedelé Bi S, Oates JF (2019) Cercopithecus diana: The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2019: e.T4245A92384250, https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2019-3.RLTS.T4245A92384250.en.
Lê S, Josse J, Husson F (2008) FactoMineR: an R package for multivariate analysis. J Stat Softw 25:1–18. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v025.i01
Article
Google Scholar
Lemasson A, Ouattara K, Bouchet H, Zuberbühler K (2010) Speed of call delivery is related to context and caller identity in Campbell’s monkey males. Naturwissenschaften 97:1023–1027. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-010-0715-6
CAS
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Loughry WJ, Oeser M, Anderson CD, Hoogland JL (2019) The importance of individual variation in the alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs. Anim Behav 150:59–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.01.019
Article
Google Scholar
Magrath RD, Haff TM, Fallow PM, Radford AN (2015) Eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls: from mechanisms to consequences: interspecific eavesdropping. Biol Rev 90:560–586. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12122
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Magrath RD, Haff TM, Igic B (2020) Interspecific communication: gaining information from heterospecific alarm calls. In: Aubin T, Mathevon N (eds) Coding Strategies in Vertebrate Acoustic Communication. Springer International Publishing, Cham, pp 287–314
Chapter
Google Scholar
Manser MB (2001) The acoustic structure of suricates’ alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:2315–2324
CAS
Article
Google Scholar
Matsuda Goodwin R, Gonedelé Bi S, Koné I (2019) Cercopithecus campbelli: The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2020: e.T136930A92374066, https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2020-2.RLTS.T136930A92374066.en
Matsuda Goodwin R, Segniagbeto G, Wiafe E, Osei D, Koné I, Gonedelé Bi S, Oates JF (2017) Cercopithecus petaurista. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2020: e.T4225A17945536. International Union for Conservation of Nature, https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2020-2.RLTS.T4225A17945536.en
McGraw S, Minhós T, Bersacola E, Ferreira da Silva MJ, Galat-Luong A, Gonedelé Bi S, Mayhew M, Oates JF, Starin ED (2020) Piliocolobus badius: The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2020: e.T161247840A161259430. https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2020-1.RLTS.T161247840A161259430.en
McGraw WS (2000) Positional behavior of Cercopithecus petaurista. Int J Primatol 21:157–182. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005483815514
Article
Google Scholar
McGraw WS, Daegling DJ (2020) Diet, feeding behavior, and jaw architecture of Taï monkeys: Congruence and chaos in the realm of functional morphology. Evol Anthropol 29:14–28. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21799
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
McGraw WS, Zuberbühler K (2008) Socioecology, predation, and cognition in a community of West African monkeys. Evol Anthropol 17:254–266. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20179
Article
Google Scholar
McGraw WS, Zuberbühler K, Noë R (2007) Monkeys of the Taï forest: an African primate community. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Book
Google Scholar
Mielke A, Zuberbühler K (2013) A method for automated individual, species and call type recognition in free-ranging animals. Anim Behav 86:475–482. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.04.017
Article
Google Scholar
Mitani M (1991) Niche overlap and polyspecific associations among sympatric cercopithecids in the campo animal reserve, Southwestern Cameroon. Primates 32:137–151. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02381172
Article
Google Scholar
Mourthé ÍMC, Guedes D, Fidelis J, Boubli JP, Mendes SL, Strier KB (2007) Ground use by northern muriquis (Brachyteles hypoxanthus). Am J Primatol 69:706–712. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20405
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mundry R, Sommer C (2007) Discriminant function analysis with nonindependent data: consequences and an alternative. Anim Behav 74:965–976. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.12.028
Article
Google Scholar
Murphy D, Lea SEG, Zuberbühler K (2013) Male blue monkey alarm calls encode predator type and distance. Anim Behav 85:119–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.015
Article
Google Scholar
Oates JF (1985) The Nigerian Guenon, Cercopithecus erythrogaster: ecological, behavioral, systematic and historical observations. Folia Primatol 45:25–43. https://doi.org/10.1159/000156189
Article
Google Scholar
Ouattara K, Lemasson A, Zuberbühler K (2009a) Campbell’s monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:22026–22031. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908118106
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Ouattara K, Lemasson A, Zuberbühler K (2009b) Campbell’s monkeys use affixation to alter call meaning. PLoS ONE 4:e7808. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007808
CAS
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Penner J, Fruteau C, Range F, Rödel M-O (2008) Finding a needle in a haystack: new methods of locating and working with rhinoceros vipers (Bitis rhinoceros). Herpetol Rev 39:310–314
Google Scholar
Price T, Arnold K, Zuberbühler K, Semple S (2009) Pyow but not hack calls of the male putty-nosed monkey (Cercopithcus nictitans) convey information about caller identity. Behaviour 146:871–888. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853908X396610
Article
Google Scholar
Price T, Fischer J (2014) Meaning attribution in the West African green monkey: influence of call type and context. Anim Cogn 17:277–286. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0660-9
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Price T, Wadewitz P, Cheney D, Seyfarth R, Hammerschmidt K, Fischer J (2015) Vervets revisited: a quantitative analysis of alarm call structure and context specificity. Sci Rep 5:13220. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep13220
CAS
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
R Core Team (2020) R: A language and environment for statistical computing, version 1.3.1093. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, https://www.R-project.org/
Rainey HJ, Zuberbühler K, Slater PJB (2004) Hornbills can distinguish between primate alarm calls. Proc R Soc Lond B 271:755–759. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2619
Article
Google Scholar
Ramakrishnan U, Coss RG (2000) Recognition of heterospecific alarm vocalization by Bonnet Macaques (Macaca radiata). J Comp Psychol 114:3–12. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.114.1.3
CAS
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Range F, Fischer J (2004) Vocal repertoire of Sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus atys) in the Tai National Park. Ethology 110:301–321. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.00973.x
Article
Google Scholar
Rauber R (2020) Call order within vocal sequences of meerkats contains temporary contextual and individual information. BMC Biol 18:119
Article
Google Scholar
Rowe N (1996) The pictorial guide to the living primates. Pogonias Press, East Hampton
Google Scholar
Schel AM, Candiotti A, Zuberbühler K (2010) Predator-deterring alarm call sequences in Guereza colobus monkeys are meaningful to conspecifics. Anim Behav 80:799–808. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.07.012
Article
Google Scholar
Schlenker P, Chemla E, Arnold K, Zuberbühler K (2016) Pyow-hack revisited: two analyses of Putty-nosed monkey alarm calls. Lingua 171:1–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2015.10.002
Article
Google Scholar
Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980) Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim Behav 28:1070–1094. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80097-2
Article
Google Scholar
Shalter M (1978) Localization of passerine seeet and mobbing calls by goshawks and pygmy owls. Z Tierpsychol 46:260–267. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1978.tb01448.x
Article
Google Scholar
Sherman PW (1977) Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. Science 197:1246–1253. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.197.4310.1246
CAS
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Shriner WM (1998) Yellow-bellied marmot and golden-mantled ground squirrel responses to heterospecific alarm calls. Anim Behav 55:529–536. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0623
CAS
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Shultz S (2008) Population density, breeding chronology and diet of Crowned Eagles Stephanoaetus coronatus in Taï National Park, Ivory Coast. Ibis 144:135–138. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0019-1019.2001.00005.x
Article
Google Scholar
Slater P, Rainey H, Zuberbühler K (2004) The responses of black-casqued hornbills to predator vocalisations and primate alarm calls. Behaviour 141:1263–1277. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539042729658
Article
Google Scholar
Slobodchikoff CN, Paseka A, Verdolin JL (2009) Prairie dog alarm calls encode labels about predator colors. Anim Cogn 12:435–439. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-008-0203-y
CAS
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Stensland E, Angerbjörn A, Berggren P (2003) Mixed species groups in mammals. Mammal Rev 33:205–223
Article
Google Scholar
Stephan C, Zuberbühler K (2016) Persistent females and compliant males coordinate alarm calling in Diana monkeys. Curr Biol 26:2907–2912. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.033
CAS
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Struhsaker TT (1967) Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aetthiops). In: Altmann SA (ed) Social communication among primates. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 281–324
Google Scholar
Struhsaker TT (1981) Polyspecific associations among tropical rain-forest primates. Z Tierpsychol 57:268–304. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1981.tb01928.x
Article
Google Scholar
Suzuki TN (2012) Referential mobbing calls elicit different predator-searching behaviours in Japanese great tits. Anim Behav 84:53–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.03.030
Article
Google Scholar
Suzuki TN (2014) Communication about predator type by a bird using discrete, graded and combinatorial variation in alarm calls. Anim Behav 87:59–65
Article
Google Scholar
Suzuki TN (2016) Referential calls coordinate multi-species mobbing in a forest bird community. J Ethol 34:79–84. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10164-015-0449-1
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Templeton CN (2005) Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934–1937. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1108841
CAS
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Townsend SW, Rasmussen M, Clutton-Brock T, Manser MB (2012) Flexible alarm calling in meerkats: the role of the social environment and predation urgency. Behav Ecol 23:1360–1364. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ars129
Article
Google Scholar
Venables WN, Ripley BD (2002) Modern Applied Statistics with S, 4th edn. Springer, New York
Book
Google Scholar
Vitousek MN, Adelman JS, Gregory NC, Clair JJHS (2007) Heterospecific alarm call recognition in a non-vocal reptile. Biol Lett 3:632–634. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2007.0443
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Vooren AP (1986) Nature and origin of tree and branch fall in the Taï Forest (Ivory Coast). Neth J Agric Sci 34:112–116. https://doi.org/10.18174/njas.v34i1.16823
Article
Google Scholar
Walton B, Kershenbaum A (2019) Heterospecific recognition of referential alarm calls in two species of lemur. Bioacoustics 28:592–603. https://doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2018.1509375
Article
Google Scholar
Wheeler BC (2010) Production and perception of situationally variable alarm calls in wild tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:989–1000. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-0914-3
Article
Google Scholar
Wich SA, de Vries H (2006) Male monkeys remember which group members have given alarm calls. Proc R Soc Lond B 273:735–740. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2005.3320
Article
Google Scholar
Windfelder TL (2001) Interspecific communication in mixed-species groups of tamarins: evidence from playback experiments. Anim Behav 61:1193–1201. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1701
Article
Google Scholar
Wolters S, Zuberbühler K (2003) Mixed-species associations of Diana and Campbell’s monkeys: the costs and benefits of a forest phenomenon. Behaviour 140:371–385. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853903321826684
Article
Google Scholar
Zuberbühler K (2000) Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proc R Soc Lond B 267:713–718. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1061
Article
Google Scholar
Zuberbühler K (2001) Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell’s monkeys, Cercopithecus campbelli. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 50:414–422. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650100383
Article
Google Scholar
Zuberbühler K (2002) A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Anim Behav 63:293–299. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1914
Article
Google Scholar
Zuberbühler K (2004) Effects of natural and sexual selection on the evolution of guenon loud calls. In: Glenn ME, Cords M (eds) The Guenons: Diversity and Adaptation in African Monkeys. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Boston, pp 289–306
Chapter
Google Scholar
Zuberbühler K, Jenny D (2002) Leopard predation and primate evolution. J Hum Evol 43:873–886. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.2002.0605
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Zuberbühler K, Jenny D, Bshary R (1999) The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology 105:477–490. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.1999.00396.x
Article
Google Scholar
Zuberbühler K, Noë R, Seyfarth RM (1997) Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators. Anim Behav 53:589–604. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0334
Article
Google Scholar