Skip to main content
Log in

Transfer of learning between unimanual and bimanual rhythmic movement coordination: transfer is a function of the task dynamic

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Under certain conditions, learning can transfer from a trained task to an untrained version of that same task. However, it is as yet unclear what those certain conditions are or why learning transfers when it does. Coordinated rhythmic movement is a valuable model system for investigating transfer because we have a model of the underlying task dynamic that includes perceptual coupling between the limbs being coordinated. The model predicts that (1) coordinated rhythmic movements, both bimanual and unimanual, are organised with respect to relative motion information for relative phase in the coupling function, (2) unimanual is less stable than bimanual coordination because the coupling is unidirectional rather than bidirectional, and (3) learning a new coordination is primarily about learning to perceive and use the relevant information which, with equal perceptual improvement due to training, yields equal transfer of learning from bimanual to unimanual coordination and vice versa [but, given prediction (2), the resulting performance is also conditioned by the intrinsic stability of each task]. In the present study, two groups were trained to produce 90° either unimanually or bimanually, respectively, and tested in respect to learning (namely improved performance in the trained 90° coordination task and improved visual discrimination of 90°) and transfer of learning (to the other, untrained 90° coordination task). Both groups improved in the task condition in which they were trained and in their ability to visually discriminate 90°, and this learning transferred to the untrained condition. When scaled by the relative intrinsic stability of each task, transfer levels were found to be equal. The results are discussed in the context of the perception–action approach to learning and performance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The normed forms of these state variables in the dynamics are those appropriate to model visual event perception (Bingham 2004a, b).

  2. All displays were presented and controlled by a custom MATLAB toolbox written by ADW and incorporating the Psychtoolbox (Brainard 1997; Kleiner et al. 2007; Pelli 1997, http://psychtoolbox.org). This software also recorded and analysed the data.

  3. Other coordination researchers rely on measures of mean error and variability. However, the hallmark of human coordinated rhythmic movement is that these are not independent. A common problem at unstable phases (e.g. 90°) is that people produce large errors (e.g. moving at 0° instead) but with low variability. You therefore cannot interpret variability without the error and vice versa. We use and advocate for the proportion measure because it addresses these problems; it succinctly and validly measures performance at the required relative phase (Wilson et al. 2010a, b).

References

  • Beek PJ, Bingham GP (1991) Task-specific dynamics and the study of perception and action: a reaction to von Hofsten (1989). Ecol Psychol 3(1):35–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein NA (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Pergamon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Bingham GP (1988) Task-specific devices and the perceptual bottleneck. Hum Mov Sci 7(2):225–264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham GP (1995) The role of perception in timing: feedback control in motor programming and task dynamics. In: Covey E, Hawkins H, Port R (eds) Neural representation of temporal patterns. Plenum Press, New York, pp 129–157

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham GP (2001) A perceptually driven dynamical model of rhythmic limb movement and bimanual coordination. In: Moore JD, Stenning K (eds) Proceedings of the 23rd annual conference of the cognitive science society. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 75–79

  • Bingham GP (2004a) A perceptually driven dynamical model of bimanual rhythmic movement (and phase perception). Ecol Psychol 16:45–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham GP (2004b) Another timing variable composed of state variables: phase perception and phase driven oscillators. In: Hecht H, Savelsbergh GJP (eds) Advances in psychology 135: time-to-contact. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 421–442

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham GP, Schmidt RC, Turvey MT, Rosenblum LD (1991) Task dynamics and resource dynamics in the assembly of a coordinated rhythmic activity. J Exp Psychol Hum 17(2):359–381

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham GP, Schmidt RC, Zaal FTJM (1999) Visual perception of relative phasing of human limb movements. Percept Psychophys 61:246–258

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham GP, Zaal FTJM, Shull JA, Collins D (2001) The effect of frequency on visual perception of relative phase and phase variability of two oscillating objects. Exp Brain Res 136:543–552

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics Toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buekers MJ, Bogaerts HP, Swinnen SP, Helsen WF (2000) The synchronization of human arm movements to external events. Neuro Lett 290:181–184

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • De Boer BJ, Peper CLE, Beek PJ (2013) Learning a new bimanual coordination pattern: interlimb interactions, attentional focus, and transfer. J Motor Behav 45:65–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Rugy A, Salesse R, Oullier O, Temprado JJ (2006) A neuro-mechanical model for interpersonal coordination. Biol Cybern 94:427–443

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG, Adamovich SV, Ostry DJ, Flanagan JR (1990) The origin of electromyograms: explanation based on the equilibrium point hypothesis. In: Winters JM, Woo SLY (eds) Multiple muscle systems: biomechanics and movement organization. Springer, New York, pp 195–213

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher NI (1993) Statistical analysis of circular data. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kay BA, Kelso JAS, Saltzman EI, Schöner G (1987) Space-time behavior of single and bimanual rhythmical movements: data and limit cycle model. J Exp Psychol Hum 13:178–192

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kay BA, Saltzman EL, Kelso JAS (1991) Steady-state and perturbed rhythmical movements: a dynamical analysis. J Exp Psychol Hum 17(1):183–197

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Keetch KM, Schmidt RA, Lee TD, Young DE (2005) Especial skills: their emergence with massive amounts of practice. J Exp Psychol Hum 31(5):970–978

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JS (1984) Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. Am J Physiol 246(6 Pt 2):R1000–R1004

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS, Zanone PG (2002) Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer across different effector systems. J Exp Psychol Hum 28(4):776–797

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS, Scholz JP, Schöner G (1986) Nonequilibrium phase transition in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations. Phys Lett A 118:279–284

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS, Schöner G, Scholz JP, Haken H (1987) Phase-locked modes, phase transitions and component oscillators in biological motion. Phys Scr 35:79–87

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kingdom FAA, Prins N (2009) Psychophysics: a practical introduction. Academic Press, Waltham

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D (2007) What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception, vol 36, ECVP Abstract Supplement

  • Kovacs AJ, Shea CH (2011) The learning of 90° continuous relative phase with and without Lissajous feedback: external and internally generated bimanual coordination. Acta Psychol 136(3):311–320

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs AJ, Buchanan JJ, Shea CH (2009a) Bimanual 1:1 with 90 continuous relative phase: difficult or easy! Exp Brain Res 193:129–136

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs AJ, Buchanan JJ, Shea CH (2009b) Using scanning trials to assess intrinsic coordination dynamics. Neuro Lett 455(3):162–167

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kugler PN, Turvey MT (1987) Information, natural law, and the self-assembly of rhythmic movement. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Langley DJ, Zelaznik HN (1984) The acquisition of time properties associated with a sequential motor skill. J Mot Behav 16(3):275–301

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maslovat D, Hodges NJ, Krigolson OE, Handy TC (2010) Observational practice benefits are limited to perceptual improvements in the acquisition of a novel coordination skill. Exp Brain Res 204:119–130

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mechsner F, Kerzel D, Knoblich G, Prinz W (2001) Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination. Nature 414:69–73

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merton PA (1972) How we control the contraction of our muscles. Sci Am 226:30–37

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM, Shapiro DC, Carlton MJ (1979) Coordinating visual and kinaesthetic memory codes. Br J Psychol 70:87–96

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proteau L, Marteniuk RG, Lévesque L (1992) A sensorimotor basis for motor learning: evidence indicating specificity of practice. Q J Exp Psychol Hum 44(A):557–575

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Saltzman E, Kelso JAS (1987) Skilled actions: a task-dynamic approach. Psychol Rev 94(1):84–106

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA (1975) A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychol Rev 82:225–260

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA, Young DE (1987) Transfer of movement control in motor skill learning. In: Cormier SM, Hagman JD (eds) Transfer of learning: contemporary research and applications. Academic Press, Orlando, pp 47–79

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RC, Carello C, Turvey MT (1990) Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. J Exp Psychol Hum 16:227–247

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simko J, Cummins F (2010) Embodied task dynamics. Psycholog Rev 117(4):1229–1246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snapp-Childs W, Wilson AD, Bingham GP (2011) The stability of rhythmic movement coordination depends on relative speed: the Bingham model supported. Exp Brain Res 215(2):89–100

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swinnen SP, De Pooter A, Delrue S (1991) Moving away from the in-phase attractor during bimanual oscillations. In: Beek PJ, Bootsma RJ, van Wieringen PCW (eds) Studies in perception and action. Rodopi, Amsterdam, pp 315–319

    Google Scholar 

  • Swinnen SP, Jardin K, Meulenbroek R, Dounskaia N, Den Brandt MHV (1997) Egocentric and allocentric constraints in the expression of patterns of interlimb coordination. J Cogn Neuro 9(3):348–377

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Swinnen SP, Jardin K, Verschueren S, Meulenbroek R, Franz L, Dounskaia N, Walter CB (1998) Exploring interlimb constraints during bimanual graphic performance: effects of muscle grouping and direction. Behav Brain Res 90(1):79–87

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Temprado JJ, Laurent M (2004) Attentional load-associated with performing and stabilizing a between-persons coordination of rhythmic limb movements. Acta Psychol 115:1–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temprado JJ, Swinnen SP, Carson RG, Tourment A, Laurent M (2003) Interaction of directional, neuromuscular and egocentric constraints on the stability of preferred bimanual coordination patterns. Hum Mov Sci 22:339–363

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike EL (1913) Educational psychology, Columbia University Press, New York

  • Walker E, Nowacki AS (2011) Understanding equivalence and noninferiority testing. J Gen Intern Med 26(2):192–196

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warren WH (2006) The dynamics of perception and action. Psycholog Rev 113(2):358–389

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson AD, Bingham GP (2008) Identifying the information for the visual perception of relative phase. Atten Percept Psychophys 70(3):465–476

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson AD, Bingham GP, Craig JC (2003) Proprioceptive perception of phase variability. J Exp Psychol Hum 29:1179–1190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson AD, Collins DR, Bingham GP (2005a) Human movement coordination implicates relative direction as the information for relative phase. Exp Brain Res 165:351–361

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson AD, Collins DR, Bingham GP (2005b) Perceptual coupling in rhythmic movement coordination—stable perception leads to stable action. Exp Brain Res 164:517–528

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson AD, Snapp-Childs W, Bingham GP (2010a) Perceptual learning immediately yields new stable motor coordination. J Exp Psychol Hum 36:1508–1514

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson AD, Snapp-Childs W, Coats R, Bingham GP (2010b) Task appropriate augmented feedback for training novel coordinated rhythmic movements. Exp Brain Res 205:513–520

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmers RH, Beek PJ, van Wieringen PCW (1992) Phase transitions in rhythmic tracking movements: a case of unilateral coupling. Hum Mov Sci 11:217–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaal FTJM, Bingham GP, Schmidt RC (2000) Visual perception of mean relative phase and phase variability. J Exp Psychol Human 26:1209–1220

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zanone PG, Kelso JAS (1992a) Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. J Exp Psychol Hum 18:403–421

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zanone PG, Kelso JAS (1992b) Learning and transfer as dynamical paradigms for behavioral change. In: Stelmach G, Requin J (eds) Tutorials in motor behavior II, advances in psychology. North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp 563–581

    Google Scholar 

  • Zanone PG, Kelso JAS (1997) Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer: collective and component levels. J Exp Psychol Hum 23:1454–1480

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was partially supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development 1R01HD070832-01 and the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Training Grant T32DC00012.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Winona Snapp-Childs.

Appendix: Additional measures of coordination performance

Appendix: Additional measures of coordination performance

Measures of mean error and variability have been used in some studies to evaluate coordination performance and learning. We report these measures and show that they are difficult to interpret in the current context in contrast to the proportion of time-on-task (PTT) measure that we have used. Similar to Maslovat et al. (2010), we computed the relative phase distributions windowed at intervals of 20° ranging from 0° to 180° and produced a histogram showing where participants were spending time when trying to move at 90° both before and after training (see Fig. 4a, b). We used this graph to interpret the mean error and variability.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Relative phase distributions for baseline and post-training for bimanual 90° separated by group: a) baseline; b) post-training

The problem for the measures is as follows. As participants begin to try to perform 90° coordination, they often fail to remain in the neighbourhood of 90° and transition to spend significant time at either 0° or 180°. As they learn and improve in performance, they succeed better in staying near or at 90° (as shown directly by the PTT measure) although they may still occasionally transition to 0° or 180°. There are individual differences in whether a performer tends to transition either to 0° or to 180° or to both. If it is both rather than just 0° or 180°, for instance, then the resultant overall variability can be increased. However, this is not relevant to the level of success in performing the task, which is to stay at or near 90°. It is all the same if the movement is at 0° or 180° instead of 90°. Also, if the performer spends similar amounts of time at 0° and at 180°, then the mean can be 90°, whereas if the performer transitions more reliably to 0°, then the mean can biased towards 0°. Again, these differences are not of direct relevance to the success in performing the task. For these reasons, measures of mean error and variability are problematic for evaluating performance in this learning task.

First, we describe the relevant measures of mean error and variability.

Data analysis

Relative phase is a circular variable (the distribution of possible values lies on a circle) that creates a problem for computing standard means and standard deviations. Circular statistics provide trigonometric solutions to these problems by treating each data point in a relative phase time series as a vector of unit length and an orientation that matches the relative phase at that time point. Mean direction is effectively the result of concatenating these vectors and computing the orientation of the vector between the origin and the tip of the final data point vector. The mean vector length or uniformity (U) (Fisher 1993) measures the variability as the length of the resultant vector divided by the number of data points (and which therefore ranges from 0 to 1). This latter was transformed into a linear variable (SDψ) that varies between 0 and infinity using the following transformation:

$${\text{SD}}\psi {\, =\, }\left( { - 2\log_{\text{e}} U} \right)^{1/2}$$

Results

First, to examine performance before and after training, we computed relative phase distributions (that is, the proportion of time spent at relative phases between 0° and 180° using 20° bins) by condition (unimanual 90°, bimanual 90°) and separated by group. We illustrate the resulting individual differences in Fig. 4. When performing the bimanual task at baseline, as expected, neither training group consistently produced a relative phase at or near 90°. As shown in Fig. 4a, the group that would subsequently be trained at the bimanual task tended to transition to and spend time at 180°, while the group that would be trained at the unimanual task tended to transition to and spend time at 0°. This was merely an individual difference between the groups that was, however, reflected in the pattern of results for the mean direction at baseline. (Note that individual differences also appeared in results at baseline for the unimanual task.) As shown in Fig. 5a, the training groups exhibited significant differences in mean direction that reflected the individual differences. To analyse the mean direction, we used a two-way mixed-design analysis of variance (ANOVA) with group (unimanual training, bimanual training) as a between-subjects factor and condition (unimanual 90°, bimanual 90°) as a within-subject factor. The result was a significant main effect of group (F (1,12) = 4.98, p < .05). However, this difference was not relevant to the level of success in performing the task to be learned. Accordingly, we had found no differences when performance was evaluated using the PTT measure of success in performing the 90° task.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Mean vector direction (in degrees) at baseline and post-training separated by condition and group: a) baseline, bimanual versus unimanual 90°; b) post-training, bimanual versus unimanual 90°

We used the same ANOVA design to analyse SDψ and found no significant main effects or interactions. This indicated that there was no difference in consistency between the groups at baseline as shown Fig. 6a. (Note that there could have been a difference if participants in one of the groups had tended to transition equally often both to 0° and to 180°, but this difference, if significant, also would not have been relevant to the evaluation of success in performing the task to be learned.)

Fig. 6
figure 6

SDψ at baseline and post-training separated by condition and group: a) baseline, bimanual versus unimanual 90°; b) post-training, bimanual versus unimanual 90°

Next, we analysed mean direction and SDψ at post-test. For mean direction, as shown in Fig. 5b, there were no significant main effects or interactions. However, as shown in Fig. 4b, the unimanually trained group still spent more time at 0° (in the bimanual task), while the bimanually trained group spent more time at 90°. This yielded a result in the analysis of SDψ where there was a significant group by condition interaction (F (1, 12) = 7.82, p < 0.02) as shown in Fig. 6b. A comparison of baseline and post-test yielded a main effect of session for SDψ (F (1,12) = 13.50, p < 0.05), but not for mean direction. Nevertheless, both measures must be taken into account when evaluating success in learning this task. The reason is that stable but highly inaccurate performance can result from spending time only at 0° or only at 180° and that apparently accurate but highly unstable performance can result from spending equal time at 0° and at 180°.

So finally, using the two measures (mean direction and SDψ), it remained unclear how to evaluate the relative transfer of training, appropriately scaled by intrinsic differences in stability between the tasks. PTT measures the goal of the learning task directly, providing a single measure of success in performing the 90° task. It also yielded good measures of transfer. Thus, this was the preferable measure to use.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Snapp-Childs, W., Wilson, A.D. & Bingham, G.P. Transfer of learning between unimanual and bimanual rhythmic movement coordination: transfer is a function of the task dynamic. Exp Brain Res 233, 2225–2238 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4292-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4292-y

Keywords

Navigation