Skip to main content
Log in

Outsider interference: no role for motor lateralization in determining the strength of avoidance responses during reaching

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

When reaches are performed toward target objects, the presence of other non-target objects influences kinematic parameters of the reach. A typical observation has been that non-targets positioned ipsilaterally to the acting limb interfere more with the trajectory of the hand than contralateral non-targets. Here, we investigate whether this effect is mediated by motor lateralization or by the relative positioning of the objects with reference to the acting limb. Participants were asked to perform reaches toward physical target objects with their preferred or non-preferred hands while physical non-targets were present in different possible positions in the workspace. We tested both left-handers and right-handers. Our results show that a participant’s handedness does not influence reaching behavior in an obstacle avoidance paradigm. Furthermore, no statistically significant differences between the use of the preferred and non-preferred hand were observed on the kinematic parameters of the reaches. We found evidence that non-targets positioned on the outside of the reaching limb influenced the reaching behavior more strongly than non-targets on the inside. Moreover, the type of movement also appeared to play a role, as reaches that crossed the workspace had a stronger effect on avoidance behavior than reaches that were ‘uncrossed.’ We interpret these results as support for the hypothesis that the avoidance response is determined by keeping a preferred distance between the acting limb in all stages of its reach toward the target and the non-target position. This process is not biased by hand dominance or the hand preference of the actor.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Chapman and Goodale (2010) showed that avoidance responses were greater with one obstacle than with two obstacles. The fact that two obstacles were present in some studies means that the avoidance response in those cases might have been constrained. In bold strokes, the avoidance responses to a primary obstacle may have been smaller in order to properly avoid the secondary obstacle. We consider that although the magnitude of the avoidance response may have been smaller with two obstacles present the direction of the effect is still quite systematic, in that ipsilateral obstacles still evoke a stronger response than contralateral obstacles.

  2. Unless specifically stated, otherwise all measures were computed from index finger marker data.

References

  • Annett J, Annett M, Hudson PTW, Turner A (1979) Control of movement in the preferred and non-preferred hands. Q J Exp Psychol-A 31:641–652

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ansuini C, Tognin V, Turella L, Castiello U (2007) Distractor objects affect fingers’ angular distances but not fingers’ shaping during grasping. Exp Brain Res 178:194–205. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0723-0

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buckingham G, Carey DP (2009) Rightward biases during bimanual reaching. Exp Brain Res 194:197–206. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1689-x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carson RG, Goodman D, Chua R, Elliott D (1993) Asymmetries in the regulation of visually guided aiming. J Mot Behav 25:21–32. doi:10.1080/00222895.1993.9941636

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Castiello U (1996) Grasping a fruit: selection for action. J Exp Psychol Human 22:582–603

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman CS, Goodale MA (2008) Missing in action: the effect of obstacle position and size on avoidance while reaching. Exp Brain Res 191:83–97. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1499-1

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman CS, Goodale MA (2010) Seeing all the obstacles in your way: the effect of visual feedback and visual feedback schedule on obstacle avoidance while reaching. Exp Brain Res 202:363–375. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2140-7

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cisek P, Kalaska JF (2010) Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annu Rev Neurosci 33:269–298

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cousineau D (2005) Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: a simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutor Quant Method Psychol 1(1):42–45

    Google Scholar 

  • Dean J, Bruwer M (1994) Control of human arm movements in two dimensions: paths and joint control in avoiding simple linear obstacles. Exp Brain Res 97:497–514

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott D, Roy EA, Goodman D et al (1993) Asymmetries in the preparation and control of manual aiming movements. Can J Exp Psychol 47:570–589. doi:10.1037/h0078856

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG, Levin MF (2009) The equilibrium-point hypothesis—past, present and future. In: Sternad D (ed) Progress in motor control: a multidisciplinary perspective. Springer, New York, pp 699–726

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Goble DJ, Brown SH (2008) The biological and behavioral basis of upper limb asymmetries in sensorimotor performance. Neurosci Biobehav R 32:598–610. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.10.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meegan DV, Tipper SP (1998) Reaching into cluttered visual environments: spatial and temporal influences of distracting objects. Q J Exp Psychol-A 51A:225–249

    Google Scholar 

  • Meegan DV, Tipper SP (1999) Visual search and target-direction action. J Exp Psychol Human 25:1–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menger R, Van der Stigchel S, Dijkerman HC (2012) How obstructing is an obstacle? The influence of starting posture on obstacle avoidance. Acta Psychol 141:1–8. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.06.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mon-Williams M, McIntosh RD (2000) A test between two hypotheses and a possible third way for the control of prehension. Exp Brain Res 134:268–273. doi:10.1007/s002210000479

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mon-Williams M, Tresilian JR, Coppard VL, Carson RG (2001) The effect of obstacle position on reach-to-grasp movements. Exp Brain Res 137:497–501. doi:10.1007/s002210100684

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mutha PK, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL (2013) Rethinking motor lateralization: specialized but complementary mechanisms for motor control of each arm. PLoS One 8(3):e58582. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058582

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt J, Abrams RA (1994) Action-centered inhibition: effects of distracters on movement planning and execution. Hum Movement Sci 13:245–254

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice NJ, McIntosh RD, Schindler I et al (2006) Intact automatic avoidance of obstacles in patients with visual form agnosia. Exp Brain Res 174:176–188. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0435-5

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roy E, Kalbfleisch L, Elliott D (1994) Kinematic analyses of manual asymmetries in visual aiming movements. Brain Cognition 24:289–295. doi:10.1006/brcg.1994.1017

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sainburg RL, Schaefer SY (2004) Interlimb differences in control of movement extent. J Neurophysiol 92:1374–1383. doi:10.1152/jn.00181.2004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schindler I, Rice NJ, McIntosh RD et al (2004) Automatic avoidance of obstacles is a dorsal stream function: evidence from optic ataxia. Nature Neurosci 7:779–784. doi:10.1038/nn1273

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smeets J, Brenner E (1995) Perception and action are based on the same visual information: distinction between position and velocity. J Exp Psychol Human 21:19–31

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Striemer CL, Chapman CS, Goodale MA (2009) “Real-time” obstacle avoidance in the absence of primary visual cortex. Proc Nat Acad Sci 106:15996–16001. doi:10.1073/pnas.0905549106

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper SP, Lortie C, Baylis GC (1992) Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. J Exp Psychol Human 18:891–905

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper SP, Howard LA, Jackson SR (1997) Selective reaching to grasp: evidence for distractor interference effects. Vis Cogn 4:1–38. doi:10.1080/713756749

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Todor JI, Kyprie PM, Price HL (1982) Lateral asymmetries in arm, wrist and finger movements. Cortex 18:515–523

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tresilian JR (1998) Attention in action or obstruction of movement? A kinematic analysis of avoidance behavior in prehension. Exp Brain Res 120:352–368

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tresilian JR, Mon-Williams M, Coppard VL, Carson RG (2005) Developmental changes in the response to obstacles during prehension. J Motor Behav 37:103–110

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh TN (2011) The relationship between attentional capture and deviations in movement trajectories in a selective reaching task. Acta Psychol 137:300–308. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.03.011

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth RS (1899) The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychol Rev Monogr Suppl 3:1–114

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rudmer Menger.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Menger, R., Van der Stigchel, S. & Dijkerman, H.C. Outsider interference: no role for motor lateralization in determining the strength of avoidance responses during reaching. Exp Brain Res 229, 533–543 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3615-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3615-0

Keywords

Navigation