Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Correlation properties of spontaneous motor activity in healthy infants: a new computer-assisted method to evaluate neurological maturation

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Qualitative assessment of spontaneous motor activity in early infancy is widely used in clinical practice. It enables the description of maturational changes of motor behavior in both healthy infants and infants who are at risk for later neurological impairment. These assessments are, however, time-consuming and are dependent upon professional experience. Therefore, a simple physiological method that describes the complex behavior of spontaneous movements (SMs) in infants would be helpful. In this methodological study, we aimed to determine whether time series of motor acceleration measurements at 40–44 weeks and 50–55 weeks gestational age in healthy infants exhibit fractal-like properties and if this self-affinity of the acceleration signal is sensitive to maturation. Healthy motor state was ensured by General Movement assessment. We assessed statistical persistence in the acceleration time series by calculating the scaling exponent α via detrended fluctuation analysis of the time series. In hand trajectories of SMs in infants we found a mean α value of 1.198 (95 % CI 1.167–1.230) at 40–44 weeks. Alpha changed significantly (p = 0.001) at 50–55 weeks to a mean of 1.102 (1.055–1.149). Complementary multilevel regression analysis confirmed a decreasing trend of α with increasing age. Statistical persistence of fluctuation in hand trajectories of SMs is sensitive to neurological maturation and can be characterized by a simple parameter α in an automated and observer-independent fashion. Future studies including children at risk for neurological impairment should evaluate whether this method could be used as an early clinical screening tool for later neurological compromise.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baldwin DN, Suki B, Pillow JJ, Roiha HL, Minocchieri S, Frey U (2004) Effect of sighs on breathing memory and dynamics in healthy infants. J Appl Physiol 97:1830–1839

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bernhardt I, Marbacher M, Hilfiker R, Radlinger L (2011) Inter- and intra-observer agreement of Prechtl’s method on the qualitative assessment of general movements in preterm, term and young infants. Early Hum Dev 87(9):633–639

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bland JM, Altman DG (1986) Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement. Lancet 1:307–310

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bos AF, van Loon AJ, Martijn A, van Asperen RM, Okken A, Prechtl HF (1997) Spontaneous motility in preterm, small-for-gestational age infants I. Quantitative aspects. Early Hum Dev 50:115–129

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Burger M, Louw QA (2009) The predictive validity of general movements—a systematic review. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 13:408–420

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cernelc M, Suki B, Reinmann B, Hall GL, Frey U (2002) Correlation properties of tidal volume and end-tidal O2 and CO2 concentrations in healthy infants. J Appl Physiol 92:1817–1827

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cioni G, Prechtl HF, Ferrari F, Paolicelli PB, Einspieler C, Roversi MF (1997) Which better predicts later outcome in full-term infants: quality of general movements or neurological examination? Early Hum Dev 50:71–85

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Constantine W, Percival D (2012) Fractal: Fractal Time Series Modeling and Analysis. R package version 1.1-1. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=fractal

  • Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP (2000) Nonlinear time series analysis of normal and pathological human walking. Chaos 10:848–863

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP (2010) Re-interpreting detrended fluctuation analyses of stride-to-stride variability in human walking. Gait Posture 32:348–353

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Disselhorst-Klug C, Heinze F, Breitbach-Faller N, Schmitz-Rode T, Rau G (2012) Introduction of a method for quantitative evaluation of spontaneous motor activity development with age in infants. Exp Brain Res 218:305–313

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Droit S, Boldrini A, Cioni G (1996) Rhythmical leg movements in low-risk and brain-damaged preterm infants. Early Hum Dev 44:201–213

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Einspieler C, Prechtl HF (2005) Prechtl’s assessment of general movements: a diagnostic tool for the functional assessment of the young nervous system. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 11:61–67

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Einspieler C, Prechtl HF, Ferrari F, Cioni G (2004) Prechtl’s method on the qualitative assessment of general movements in preterm. Cambridge University Press, Term and Young Infants

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari F, Cioni G, Prechtl HF (1990) Qualitative changes of general movements in preterm infants with brain lesions. Early Hum Dev 23:193–231

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gates DH, Dingwell JB (2007) Peripheral neuropathy does not alter the fractal dynamics of gait stride intervals. J Appl Physiol 102(3):965–971

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gima H, Ohgi S, Morita S, Karasuno H, Fujiwara T, Abe K (2011) A dynamical system analysis of the development of spontaneous lower extremity movements in newborn and young infants. J Physiol Anthropol 30(5):179–186

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberger AL, Amaral LA, Hausdorff JM, Ivanov PC, Peng CK, Stanley HE (2002) Fractal dynamics in physiology: alterations with disease and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99(Suppl 1):2466–2472

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hausdorff JM, Peng CK, Ladin Z, Wei JY, Goldberger AL (1995) Is walking a random walk? Evidence for long-range correlations in stride interval of human gait. J Appl Physiol 78:349–358

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hausdorff JM, Purdon PL, Peng CK, Ladin Z, Wei JY, Goldberger AL (1996) Fractal dynamics of human gait: stability of long-range correlations in stride interval fluctuations. J Appl Physiol 80:1448–1457

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hausdorff JM, Mitchell SL, Firtion R, Peng CK, Cudkowicz ME, Wei JY, Goldberger AL (1997) Altered fractal dynamics of gait: reduced stride-interval correlations with aging and Huntington’s disease. J Appl Physiol 82:262–269

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hausdorff JM, Zemany L, Peng C, Goldberger AL (1999) Maturation of gait dynamics: stride-to-stride variability and its temporal organization in children. J Appl Physiol 86:1040–1047

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Karch D, Kim KS, Wochner K, Pietz J, Dickhaus H, Philippi H (2008) Quantification of the segmental kinematics of spontaneous infant movements. J Biomech 41:2860–2867

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karch D, Wochner K, Kim K, Philippi H, Hadders-Algra M, Pietz J, Dickhaus H (2010) Quantitative score for the evaluation of kinematic recordings in neuropediatric diagnostics. Detection of complex patterns in spontaneous limb movements. Methods Inf Med 49:526–530

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ligges U (2011) Tune R: Analysis of music. http://r-forge.r-project.org/projects/tuner/

  • Maraun D, Rust HW, Timmer J (2004) Tempting long-memory—on the interpretation of DFA results. Nonlinear Process Geophys 11:495–503

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meinecke L, Breitbach-Faller N, Bartz C, Damen R, Rau G, Disselhorst-Klug C (2006) Movement analysis in the early detection of newborns at risk for developing spasticity due to infantile cerebral palsy. Hum Mov Sci 25:125–144

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ohgi S, Morita S, Loo KK, Mizuike C (2007) A dynamical systems analysis of spontaneous movements in newborn infants. J Mot Behav 39:203–214

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohgi S, Morita S, Loo KK, Mizuike C (2008) Time series analysis of spontaneous upper-extremity movements of premature infants with brain injuries. Phys Ther 88:1022–1033

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ottaviano S, Guidetti V, Allemand F, Spinetoli B, Seri S (1989) Laterality of arm movement in full-term newborn. Early Hum Dev 19:3–7

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Peng CK, Mietus J, Hausdorff JM, Havlin S, Stanley HE, Goldberger AL (1993) Long-range anticorrelations and non-gaussian behavior of the heartbeat. Phys Rev Lett 70:1343–1346

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng CK, Havlin S, Stanley HE, Goldberger AL (1995) Quantification of scaling exponents and crossover phenomena in nonstationary heartbeat time-series. Chaos 5:82–87

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Piek JP (2001) Is a quantitative approach useful in the comparison of spontaneous movements in fullterm and preterm infants? Hum Mov Sci 20:717–736

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Piek JP, Carman R (1994) Developmental profiles of spontaneous movements in infants. Early Hum Dev 39:109–126

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Prechtl HF (1974) The behavioural states of the newborn infant (a review). Brain Res 76:185–212

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team (2012) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria (2012). ISBN 3-900051-07-0, http://www.R-project.org

  • Schreiber T, Schmitz A (2000) Surrogate time series. Physics 142(3–4):346–382

    Google Scholar 

  • Spittle AJ, Doyle LW, Boyd RN (2008) A systematic review of the clinimetric properties of neuromotor assessments for preterm infants during the first year of life. Dev Med Child Neurol 50:254–266

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stern G, Beel J, Suki B, Silverman M, Westaway J, Cernelc M, Baldwin D, Frey U (2009) Long-range correlations in rectal temperature fluctuations of healthy infants during maturation. PLoS One 4:e6431

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thamrin C, Frey U (2009) Complexity and respiratory growth: a developing story. J Appl Physiol 106:753–754

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thamrin C, Stern G, Frey U (2010) Fractals for physicians. Paediatr Respir Rev 11:123–131

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Theiler J, Eubank S, Longtin A, Galdrikian B, Farmer JD (1992) Testing for nonlinearity in time series: the method of surrogate data. Phys D 58(1–4):77–94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaal J, Van Soest AJ, Hopkins B, Sie LT, Van der Knaap MS (2000) Development of spontaneous leg movements in infants with and without periventricular leukomalacia. Exp Brain Res 135:94–105

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Heide J, Paolicelli PB, Boldrini A, Cioni G (1999) Kinematic and qualitative analysis of lower extremitymovements in preterm infants with brain lesions. Phys Ther 79:546–557

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank PD Dr. Matthias Nelle (Department of Neonatology, University Children’s Hospital, Bern Switzerland), Brigitte Püntener and Prof. Dr. Lorenz Radlinger (University of Applied Sciences, Health, Bern, Switzerland) for their support.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sandra Waldmeier.

Additional information

Sandra Waldmeier, Sebastian Grunt has been contributed equally.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA)

Detrended fluctuation analysis has been introduced in medical research to assess vital signals and biofeedback systems. DFA is an extension of fluctuation analysis of non-stationary time series (a signal whose mean and variance dynamics change over time) of stochastic processes to determine self-affinity of the signal and uncover fractal like properties. DFA was implemented according to Peng et al. (1995) as follows: The time series was first integrated and then divided into non-overlapping windows of size n. For each window, a least square first-order approximation was obtained which represents the “trend” in the signal. This first approximation of the integrated signal was denoted y_n(k). By subtracting y_n(k) from the integrated signal y(k), the detrended signal was obtained. This is the approximation error e n (k) for which the root mean squared error was obtained and denoted

$$ {F_n = }{\sqrt {{\frac {1} {N}} {\sum_{k=1}^{N} {e_n} {{(k)}^2}}}} $$

where N is the number of intervals in the signal. Fn was obtained for several different window lengths n. With increasing window length, the approximation error increased: Log (n) versus log (Fn) gave a monotonically increasing relation that in the context of physiological signals has been found to be linear with slope alpha. Previous research has provided evidence for fractal characteristics in the signal: Fn and n are related via a power-law, thus the linear characteristic of the log–log relation. This scaling behaviour can then be precisely characterized by the value of alpha.

While Peng et al. (1993) have generally interpreted DFA as a measure of long-range correlations, recently Dingwell et al. (2010) and Maraun et al. (2004) proposed the interpretation of DFA as an indicator of persistence and anti-persistence (see “Introduction”). Herein we have followed their approach, thus, our results are interpretated in terms of persistence and anti-persistence of the acceleration time series.

An accelerometer time series with an α of 0.5 indicates a system that is not deterministic and is prone to instabilities, whereas higher α value implies more deterministic behavior with stronger correlations between an accelerated movement and its preceding accelerated movements (=persistence of an acceleration time series).

Appendix 2: Statistical tests using surrogate time series

Following Dingwell and Cusumano (2010), we generated for each measured time series one thousand randomly shuffled surrogates by independently permuting the order of the values in the time series (Hausdorff et al. 1995; Gates and Dingwell 2007; Theiler et al. 1992). These surrogates were used to test the hypothesis that the participants controlled their movements by sending signals to their muscles that are temporally independent from previous signals. The distribution of alpha values displayed by the surrogate time series clearly allows for a rejection of this hypothesis for each participant at a significance level of α = 0.05 (see Tables 5, 6), after correcting the p values for multiple comparison using Bonferroni’s method.

Table 5 Shuffling surrogate analysis MS I
Table 6 Shuffling surrogate analysis MS II

Dingwell and Cusumano (2010) demonstrated that phase-randomized surrogates of stride-length time series and of stride-time time series obtained from young healthy adults walking on a motorized treadmill cannot be distinguished from the original time series in terms of detrended fluctuation analysis. Thereby, the hypothesis cannot be rejected, that subjects independently choose their stride-time and stride-length as temporally correlated auto-regressive (AR) process (Theiler et al. 1992; Schreiber and Schmitz 2000), or more generally as nearly equivalent auto-regressive moving-average (ARMA) process (Theiler et al., p. 81). We investigated to what extent this result applies to time series of motor acceleration measurements in healthy infants (at 40–44 and 50–55 weeks gestational age). Accordingly, we generated for each measured time series one thousand phase-randomized surrogates. That is, for each time series, we computed the Fourier transform, randomized the phase spectrum, and then computed the inverse Fourier transform (Theiler et al. 1992; Schreiber and Schmitz 2000; Dingwell and Cusumano 2000). These surrogates preserve the power spectra and auto-correlation properties of each original time series, thus preserving their statistical persistence.

Based on our results, for almost all participants, the aforementioned hypothesis, now concerning the subjects’ limb acceleration patterns, cannot be rejected. Tables 7 and 8 list the resulting p values from our statistical analysis using phase-randomized surrogate data for MS I and MS II.

Table 7 Phase-randomized surrogate analysis MS I
Table 8 Phase-randomized surrogate analysis MS II

The present work focuses on statistically significant changes in the alpha value of time series of motor acceleration measurements taken at 40–44 and at 50–55 weeks of gestational age and their potential interpretation as an indicator of maturational changes of motor behavior. However, we considered it pertinent to conduct similar tests as the ones performed in Dingwell and Cusumano (2010) in order to investigate potential analogies in the properties of the time series under consideration.

All calculations were conducted in R (R Core Team 2012).

Appendix 3: Sampling frequency issues during measurements

In order to study potential issues with the method of detrended fluctuation analysis that could arise from a low sampling frequency during the measurement, we conducted a simulation study. We investigated the effect of applying detrended fluctuation analysis to a time series obtained from an “original” time series by sampling the time series with a lower sampling rate. This sampling procedure boils down to leaving out one or more consecutive values from the original time series in a periodic manner.

The “original” time series consisted of pink noise generated via simulation. It can be theoretically shown that the alpha value obtained from such a time series should be approximately equal to 1. We generated one thousand such time series and with each one we conducted the sampling procedure with a fixed sampling rate. Different sampling rates were used in order to assess the strength of the effect as the sampling frequency is lowered. For a given sampling rate, the aforementioned procedure yielded one thousand shorter time series. We then compared the alpha value of the original time series with the alpha value of the series obtained via the sampling procedure. We observed a change of the alpha value, which was consistently in the same direction. The distribution of the differences in the alpha values is displayed in Fig. 4.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Histograms of absolute errors due to subsampling. The distribution of the differences in the alpha values due to sampling frequency issues. Every panel corresponds to a different sampling frequency. The number of data points left out between recorded values are: Upper row, from left to right 1, 2, 3, 4. Lower Row, from left to right 10, 20, 50, 100

Since the sampling procedure generates shorter time series, we also generated additional one thousand pink noise time series of the same length as the ones resulting from the sampling procedure. We compared the distributions of the alpha values in each of the two groups observing a statistically significant difference (p values ≤1.705e−10, Mann–Whitney test, p values ≤1.866e−8, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test).

The mean strength of the effect varied with the sampling frequency used. The introduced relative error (in percentage) ranges from 4.65 %, when leaving out every other value, to 19.37 %, when only every hundredth value is considered (see Fig. 5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Relative error due to subsampling

Fortunately, the effect is consistent in direction, suggesting that the errors in the alpha values caused by a low sampling rate during measurement should not jeopardize the validity of our main results. Of central importance for our study is the change in the alpha values during development. While the interpretation of the actual absolute alpha values might be interesting (and controversial, as pointed out in the literature, see, for instance, Maraun et al. 2004), we focused herein more on the change of this value during development. We believe that, as long as we consistently use the same sampling frequency during all measurements, the reported changes in the value of alpha properly quantify the changes in the statistical persistence and anti-persistence properties of the sampled time series. Consequently, the reported changes in the value of alpha might represent a legitimate way of systematically quantifying neurological maturation.

All calculations were conducted in R (R Core Team 2012).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Waldmeier, S., Grunt, S., Delgado-Eckert, E. et al. Correlation properties of spontaneous motor activity in healthy infants: a new computer-assisted method to evaluate neurological maturation. Exp Brain Res 227, 433–446 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3504-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3504-6

Keywords

Navigation