Abstract
Rationale
Animal studies indicate that dopamine pathways in the ventral striatum code for the motivational salience of both rewarding and aversive stimuli, but evidence for this mechanism in humans is less established. We have developed a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) model which permits examination of the neural processing of both rewarding and aversive stimuli.
Objectives
The aim of the study was to determine the effect of the dopamine receptor antagonist, sulpiride, on the neural processing of rewarding and aversive stimuli in healthy volunteers.
Methods
We studied 30 healthy participants who were randomly allocated to receive a single dose of sulpiride (400 mg) or placebo, in a double-blind, parallel-group design. We used fMRI to measure the neural response to rewarding (taste or sight of chocolate) and aversive stimuli (sight of mouldy strawberries or unpleasant strawberry taste) 4 h after drug treatment.
Results
Relative to placebo, sulpiride reduced blood oxygenation level-dependent responses to chocolate stimuli in the striatum (ventral striatum) and anterior cingulate cortex. Sulpiride also reduced lateral orbitofrontal cortex and insula activations to the taste and sight of the aversive condition.
Conclusions
These results suggest that acute dopamine receptor blockade modulates mesolimbic and mesocortical neural activations in response to both rewarding and aversive stimuli in healthy volunteers. This effect may be relevant to the effects of dopamine receptor antagonists in the treatment of psychosis and may also have implications for the possible antidepressant properties of sulpiride.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
Recent advances in neuroimaging have allowed the identification of the neural systems activated during reward processing and motivational behaviour in humans (Knutson and Cooper 2005). Studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have revealed increased activity in the ventral striatum during the presentation of primary or secondary rewarding stimuli such as pleasant pictures, music, food, or monetary reward (Blood and Zatorre 2001; Lane et al. 1997; O’Doherty et al. 2001a, b). The medial prefrontal cortex and the anterior/pregenual cingulate are also reported to be important in the neural response to pleasant stimuli and reward-motivated behaviour (Bechara et al. 1996; Kahnt et al. 2010; Kringelbach et al. 2003; Rolls et al. 2003b, 2010; Rolls and McCabe 2007). Conversely, aversive stimuli have been found to activate the lateral orbitofrontal cortices, the amygdala and caudate nuclei (Fitzgerald et al. 2004; Rolls et al. 2003a; Zald et al. 2002).
Dopamine pathways are known to play a key role in reward-based mechanisms through dopamine release from ventral tegmental area neurons at their terminals in the nucleus accumbens, ventral striatum (Bjorklund and Dunnett 2007). Moreover, the role of dopamine extends to the processing of aversive stimuli, giving rise to the notion that dopamine pathways determine the motivational salience of environmental stimuli whether positively or negatively valenced (Matsumoto and Hikosaka 2009). Previous studies which have examined the influence of dopamine manipulation on the neural processes of reward and punishment in humans are consistent with this proposal. For example, (Knutson et al. 2004) found that amphetamine administration modulated prefrontal and striatal activations produced by monetary reward and loss, whilst Menon et al. (2007) reported that the dopamine receptor antagonist, haloperidol, abolished the striatal response to the prediction error involved in learning an aversive conditioned response.
We have developed an experimental paradigm which allows the study of the neural responses to primary and secondary rewarding and aversive stimuli in the human brain using fMRI. Using this approach, we have been able to show that the chocolate stimuli activate parts of the reward system such as the striatum, the medial orbitofrontal cortex and pregenual cingulate gyrus, whereas the unpleasant strawberry conditions activate the lateral orbitofrontal cortex and insula cortex (McCabe et al. 2010; Rolls and McCabe 2007). We have also shown that unmedicated patients recovered from depression have reduced ventral striatal responses to primary reward, suggesting trait abnormalities in reward mechanisms in those at risk of mood disorder (McCabe et al. 2009). The aim of the present study was to use the dopamine receptor antagonist, sulpiride, to examine the effects of dopamine D2/D3 receptor blockade on the neural responses to rewarding and aversive stimuli in our model. We hypothesised that sulpiride would lower the response in ventral striatum to chocolate reward and also diminish the neural processing of an aversive taste in lateral orbitofrontal cortex.
Materials and methods
Participants
Thirty healthy volunteers were randomised to receive a single oral dose with sulpiride (400 mg, n = 15), or placebo (n = 15), in a double-blind between-groups design. We used a between-groups design to minimise the effects of practice or habituation to the stimuli used in this study. Volunteers were also matched for age and gender (see Table 1). Ethical approval was provided by the Oxford Research Ethics Committee B and written informed consent was obtained from all participants before screening and after a complete description of the study was given. Exclusion criteria for all subjects were current or past Axis-1 disorder on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (Spitzer et al. 2004) and any contraindications to MRI, e.g. pacemaker, mechanical heart valve, hip replacement, metal implants.
None of the participants took current medication apart from the contraceptive pill. Before drug administration and to ensure group matching, baseline information was collected using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al. 1961), State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger 1983), the Fawcett–Clarke Pleasure Scale (FCPS; Fawcett et al. 1983), and the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS; Snaith et al. 1995). The participants also completed a “chocolate questionnaire” to measure liking, craving and frequency of eating chocolate (Rolls and McCabe 2007). Body mass index (BMI) was also calculated for each volunteer. To assess the effects of the treatment, the following questionnaires were taken before and after the treatment: visual analogue scales (VAS) of happiness, sadness, anger, disgust, alertness and anxiety, and the State Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger 1983). Volunteers also completed the Positive Affect Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al. 1988) before and after the treatment.
Overall design
We compared brain responses to rewarding and aversive stimuli across the two drug groups. Each of the following conditions were applied nine times in a randomised order (see Electronic supplementary material (ESM) Table S1): chocolate in the mouth, chocolate picture, chocolate in the mouth with chocolate picture, strawberry in the mouth, strawberry picture, strawberry in the mouth with strawberry picture. Subjective effects of the stimuli were measured by psychophysical ratings of “pleasantness”, “intensity” and “wanting” made on every trial by the subjects during the fMRI acquisition. The participants were instructed not to eat chocolate for 24 h before the scan and to eat only a small lunch on the day of scanning. Mood state was recorded on the study day with the BDI (Beck et al. 1961).
Stimuli
Stimuli were delivered to the subject’s mouth through three Teflon tubes (one for the tasteless rinse control described below, one for chocolate taste and one for strawberry taste); the tubes were held between the lips. Each tube was connected to a separate reservoir via a syringe and a one-way syringe activated check valve (model 14044-5, World Precision Instruments, Inc.), which allowed 0.5 ml of any stimulus to be delivered manually at the time indicated by the computer. The chocolate was formulated to be liquid at room temperature, with a list of the six stimulus conditions described in Table 1. A control tasteless solution, 0.5 mL of a saliva-like rinse solution (25 × 10−3 mol/L KCl and 2.5 × 10−3 mol/L NaHCO3 in distilled H20), was used between trials (tl in ESM Table S1); when subtracted from the effects of the other stimuli, this allowed somatosensory and any mouth movement effects to be subtracted from the effects produced by the other oral stimuli (de Araujo et al. 2003a; O’Doherty et al. 2001c). This allows the taste, texture and olfactory areas to be shown independently of any somatosensory effects produced by introducing a fluid into the mouth (de Araujo et al. 2003a, b; de Araujo and Rolls 2004; O’Doherty et al. 2001c). The aversive stimulus was a strawberry drink (Rosemount Pharmaceuticals Ltd.) which was rated as intense as the chocolate, but unpleasant in valence (McCabe et al. 2009). Both the liquid chocolate and the strawberry had approximately the same sweetness and texture, which enabled them to pass freely through the Teflon delivery tubes.
Experimental procedure
At the beginning of each trial, one of the six stimuli chosen by random permutation was presented. If the trial involved an oral stimulus, this was delivered in a 0.5-mL aliquot to the subject’s mouth. At the same time, at the start of the trial, a visual stimulus was presented, which was either the picture of chocolate, of mouldy strawberries or a grey control image of approximately the same intensity. The image was turned off after 7 s, at which time a small green cross appeared on a visual display to indicate to the subject to swallow what was in the mouth. After a delay of 2 s, the subject was asked to rate each of the stimuli for “pleasantness” on that trial (with +2 being very pleasant and −2 very unpleasant), for “intensity” on that trial (0 to +4) and for “wanting” (+2 for wanting very much, 0 for neutral, and −2 for very much not wanting). The ratings were made with a VAS in which the subject moved the bar to the appropriate point on the scale using a button box. After the last rating, the grey visual stimulus indicated the delivery of the tasteless control solution, which was also used as a rinse between stimuli; this was administered in exactly the same way as a test stimulus, and the subject was cued to swallow after 7 s by the green cross. The tasteless control was always accompanied by the grey visual stimulus. On trials on which only the picture of chocolate was shown, there was no rinse, but the grey visual stimulus was shown in order to allow an appropriate contrast, as described below. There was then a 2-s delay period that allowed for swallowing followed by a 1-s gap until the start of the next trial. A trial was repeated for each of the six stimulus conditions shown in ESM Table S1, and the whole cycle was repeated nine times. The instruction given to the subject was (on oral delivery trials) to move the tongue once as soon as a stimulus or tasteless solution was delivered in order to distribute the solution round the mouth to activate the receptors for taste and smell, and then to keep still for the remainder of the 7-s period until the green cross was shown, when the subject could swallow. This procedure has been shown to allow taste effects to be demonstrated clearly with fMRI using the procedure of subtracting any activations produced by the tasteless control from those produced by a taste or other stimulus (de Araujo et al. 2003a, b; de Araujo and Rolls 2004; O’Doherty et al. 2001c).
fMRI scan
The experimental protocol consisted of an event-related interleaved design using in random permuted sequence the six stimuli described above and shown in ESM Table S1. Images were acquired with a 3.0-T Varian/Siemens whole-body scanner at the Oxford Centre for Clinical Magnetic Resonance Imaging where T2*-weighted EPI slices were acquired every 2 s (TR = 2). Imaging parameters were selected to minimise susceptibility and distortion artefact in the orbitofrontal cortex (Wilson et al. 2002). Coronal slices (33) with in-plane resolution of 3 × 3 mm and between plane spacing of 4 mm were obtained. The matrix size was 64 × 64 and the field of view was 192 × 192 mm. Acquisition was carried out during the task performance, yielding 972 volumes in total. A whole brain T2*-weighted EPI volume of the above dimensions and an anatomical T1 volume with coronal plane slice thickness 3 mm and in-plane resolution of 1.0 × 1.0 mm were also acquired.
fMRI analysis
The imaging data were analysed using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Pre-processing of the data used realignment, reslicing with sinc interpolation, normalisation to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinate system and spatial smoothing with a 6-mm full width at half-maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel. Time series non-sphericity at each voxel was estimated and corrected for (Friston et al. 2002), and a high-pass filter with a cutoff period of 128 s was applied. In the single event design, a general linear model was then applied to the time course of activation where stimulus onsets were modelled as single impulse response functions and then convolved with the canonical haemodynamic response function (Friston et al. 1994). Linear contrasts were defined to test specific effects. Time derivatives were included in the basis functions set. Following smoothness estimation, linear contrasts of parameter estimates were defined to test the specific effects of each condition with each individual dataset. Voxel values for each contrast resulted in a statistical parametric map of the corresponding t statistic, which was then transformed into the unit normal distribution (SPM Z). The statistical parametric maps from each individual dataset were then entered into second-level, random effects analyses accounting for both scan-to-scan and subject-to-subject variability. SPM converts the t statistics to Z scores (Table 4). We examined simple main effects of group with one-sample t tests, for all chocolate stimuli together or all strawberry stimuli, thresholded at p = 0.001 and whole brain cluster-corrected (p < 0.05 family-wise errors (FWE) for multiple comparisons). To assess between-group differences for each condition, two-sample t tests were used, thresholded at p = 0.05 and whole brain cluster-corrected (p < 0.05 FWE for multiple comparisons). We examined between-group differences for all the positive chocolate together, all the aversive strawberry together and for each contrast separately. Plots of contrast estimates are extracted using the plots tool in SPM8. Coordinates of the activations are listed in the stereotactic space of The Montreal Neurological Institute’s ICBM152 brain (Table 2 and ESM Table S3).
Results
Demographic details and mood ratings
There were no significant differences between the two groups as determined by one-way ANOVAs for age, gender, BMI, chocolate liking and attitudes to food (EAT), all (p > 0.07, Table 1). There were no significant differences between the two groups as determined by one-way ANOVAs for measures of anhedonia (SHAPS, FCPS) or mood (BDI) (all p > 0.07, Table 1). There were no significant effects of group [F(1,27) = 1.6, p = 0.2], between the sulpiride- and placebo-treated groups on VAS (alertness, disgust, drowsiness, anxiety, happiness, nausea and sadness) or PANAS [F(1,27) = 0.36, p = 0.55], as determined by repeated measures ANOVAs (ESM Table S2).
Ratings of stimuli
Ratings of pleasantness, intensity and wanting for the stimuli were obtained during the scanning on each trial for every condition. All subjects rated the strawberry picture and taste as unpleasant and the chocolate stimuli as pleasant. Using repeated measures ANOVA with “ratings” as a first factor with three levels—pleasantness, intensity and wanting—and “condition” as a second factor with six levels (see ESM Table S1 for six condition levels), there was no significant effect of group [F(1,28) = 1.6, p = 0.2] or group × condition interaction [F(1,28) = 1.72, p = 0.2] (see ESM Fig. S1).
fMRI responses
ESM Table S3 provides a summary of the main effects of one-sample t tests for the positive chocolate stimuli and the aversive strawberry stimuli. Table 2 provides a summary of the results of the interaction with group.
Main effect of task
As expected, the positive chocolate stimuli activated reward-relevant circuitry including the ventral striatum, the putamen, the anterior insula and parts of the anterior cingulate cortex. The unpleasant strawberry stimuli activated areas involved in aversive processing including the posterior insula cortex and the lateral orbitofrontal cortex, but not the ventral striatum (ESM Table S3).
Effect of sulpiride on chocolate reward
The sulpiride group, compared to placebo, showed less blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) activation to the chocolate stimuli in the areas known to play a key role in reward, including the ventral striatum and the anterior cingulate cortex (Figs. 1a, b and 2a, b.).
Effect of sulpiride on aversive strawberry
The sulpiride group relative to placebo showed less BOLD activation to the strawberry stimuli in areas known to play a key role in processing aversive stimuli including the lateral orbitofrontal cortex and insula, as illustrated in Fig. 3. Relative to placebo, the sulpiride group also had less BOLD activation to the unpleasant strawberry taste in the precentral gyrus (Table 2).
Discussion
Our results indicate that a single dose of the D2/3 receptor antagonist, sulpiride, can decrease the neural processing of reward in the ventral striatum and anterior cingulate despite no change in subjective mood. Furthermore, we found that sulpiride decreased activation to the aversive sight and taste of mouldy strawberries in the insula and lateral orbitofrontal cortex.
The decreased striatal activation to reward seen with sulpiride is in keeping with our hypothesis that inhibition of dopamine transmission would negatively impact on the neural processing of pleasant stimuli. A recent study which examined the effects of a single dose of the antipsychotic olanzapine on neural processing of delayed incentive salience of monetary reward in healthy volunteers also reported reduced ventral striatal activation (Abler et al. 2007), whilst Knutson et al. (2004) found that the dopamine agonist amphetamine increased the duration (but decreased the peak) of ventral striatal response to win anticipation. Interestingly, a hyperactive striatal response to reward was also found in patients with schizophrenia, supporting the notion that psychosis may represent a state of abnormal salience (Kapur 2003). From this viewpoint, dopamine receptor antagonists may produce therapeutic benefit in psychosis by decreasing the tendency of patients to ascribe novelty and motivational importance to irrelevant stimuli. Unfortunately, this effect of antipsychotic drugs may also be directed towards normal motivational drives, perhaps accounting for the association of antipsychotic treatment with emotional blunting and indifference to normal social reinforcers.
We also found decreased activation to the aversive stimuli in brain regions such as the lateral orbitofrontal cortex and the insula which have been previously shown to encode the processing of unpleasant stimuli (Fitzgerald et al. 2004; Small et al. 2001) and specifically also in the current model of aversion processing (Horder et al. 2010; McCabe et al. 2009, 2010). Interestingly, therefore, it seems that antipsychotics not only have the ability to reduce the attribution of salience to rewarding stimuli but also are able to dampen the response to negatively valenced stimuli. These results therefore support the idea that dopamine is involved in the salience of both positive and negative events (Knutson et al. 2004; Schultz 2010a, b). Furthermore, recent studies examining the effects of pharmacological manipulation of dopamine in both Parkinson’s disease (PD) and Tourette’s syndrome found that dopamine enhancement in PD enhanced reward learning and dopamine blockade in Tourettes enhanced punishment avoidance (Palminteri et al. 2009). These data suggest that modulating dysfunctional dopamine systems enhances decision making by redressing the reward–punishment imbalance. Preclinical studies on the effects of antipsychotics also indicate that dopamine blockade in the striatum disrupts avoidance performance and that this may be due to the reduced activation of instrumental behaviour (Maia and Frank 2011). Moreover, studies examining behavioural strategies in response to uncertainty in schizophrenic patients implicate decreased dopamine in the prefrontal complex as underpinning such dysfunction (Strauss et al. 2011).
Reducing reactivity to and salience attribution of aversive stimuli is a common goal in treatments of disorders characterised by negative affectivity and may help explain why antipsychotic drugs are sometimes found to be beneficial in the treatment of anxiety and depression. In particular, substituted benzamides such as sulpiride and amisulpiride are commonly used clinically in some countries for the treatment of chronic mood disorders such as dysthymia (Lecrubier et al. 1997). Furthermore, a similar effect on aversive processing was also seen with 7-day administration of the SSRI citalopram in our model in healthy volunteers (McCabe et al. 2010).
Previous studies have shown that schizophrenia involves hyperactivity of dopamine neurons in the basal ganglia, but decreased activity in the prefrontal cortex (Epstein et al. 1999; Lewis 1995; Meyer-Lindenberg et al. 2002). It is therefore of great interest that we found decreased activation to the pleasant chocolate reward in the ventral striatum yet did not find any increased prefrontal cortex activity at the statistical threshold used here. It is important to assess whether such effects may be seen with a larger sample of volunteers and increased statistical power. The current study used a between-subjects, placebo-controlled design to minimise the effects of practice or habituation to the stimuli used here. In future studies, we plan to explore the effects of drug manipulations in this model using a within-subjects design to minimise inter-subject variability. Such an approach may ultimately improve statistical power and be useful in the future application of this model.
Taken together, the results from this study show that a single dose of the antipsychotic drug sulpiride can differentially modulate neural activity in response to rewarding and aversive stimuli in healthy volunteers. These findings may have implications for the mode of action of drugs such as sulpiride in the treatment of psychosis and mood disorders. Studies in patients will be required to see whether the effects we have seen are related to the therapeutic actions of sulpiride in clinical populations.
References
Abler B, Erk S, Walter H (2007) Human reward system activation is modulated by a single dose of olanzapine in healthy subjects in an event-related, double-blind, placebo-controlled fMRI study. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 191:823–833
Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR (1996) Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 6:215–225
Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J (1961) An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4:561–571
Bjorklund A, Dunnett SB (2007) Dopamine neuron systems in the brain: an update. Trends Neurosci 30:194–202
Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ (2001) Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:11818–11823
de Araujo IET, Rolls ET (2004) Representation in the human brain of food texture and oral fat. J Neurosci 24:3086–3093
de Araujo IET, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hobden P (2003a) The representation of umami taste in the human brain. J Neurophysiol 90:313–319
de Araujo IET, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, McGlone F (2003b) Human cortical responses to water in the mouth, and the effects of thirst. J Neurophysiol 90:1865–1876
Epstein J, Stern E, Silbersweig D (1999) Mesolimbic activity associated with psychosis in schizophrenia. Symptom-specific PET studies. Ann N Y Acad Sci 877:562–574
Fawcett J, Clark DC, Scheftner WA, Gibbons RD (1983) Assessing anhedonia in psychiatric patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 40:79–84
Fitzgerald DA, Posse S, Moore GJ, Tancer ME, Nathan PJ, Phan KL (2004) Neural correlates of internally-generated disgust via autobiographical recall: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Neurosci Lett 370:91–96
Friston KJ, Worsley KJ, Frackowiak RSJ, Mazziotta JC, Evans AC (1994) Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent. Hum Brain Mapp 1:214–220
Friston KJ, Glaser DE, Henson RN, Kiebel S, Phillips C, Ashburner J (2002) Classical and Bayesian inference in neuroimaging: applications. Neuroimage 16:484–512
Horder J, Harmer CJ, Cowen PJ, McCabe C (2010) Reduced neural response to reward following 7 days treatment with the cannabinoid CB(1) antagonist rimonabant in healthy volunteers. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 13:1103–1113
Kahnt T, Heinzle J, Park SQ, Haynes JD (2010) The neural code of reward anticipation in human orbitofrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:6010–6015
Kapur S (2003) Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 160:13–23
Knutson B, Cooper JC (2005) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction. Curr Opin Neurol 18:411–417
Knutson B, Bjork JM, Fong GW, Hommer D, Mattay VS, Weinberger DR (2004) Amphetamine modulates human incentive processing. Neuron 43:261–269
Kringelbach ML, O’Doherty J, Rolls ET, Andrews C (2003) Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb Cortex 13:1064–1071
Lane RD, Reiman EM, Ahern GL, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ (1997) Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. Am J Psychiatry 154:926–933
Lecrubier Y, Boyer P, Turjanski S, Rein W (1997) Amisulpride versus imipramine and placebo in dysthymia and major depression. Amisulpride Study Group. J Affect Disord 43:95–103
Lewis DA (1995) Neural circuitry of the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52:269–273, discussion 277–278
Maia TV, Frank MJ (2011) From reinforcement learning models to psychiatric and neurological disorders. Nat Neurosci 14:154–162
Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O (2009) Two types of dopamine neuron distinctly convey positive and negative motivational signals. Nature 459:837–841
McCabe C, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ (2009) Neural representation of reward in recovered depressed patients. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 205:667–677
McCabe C, Mishor Z, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ (2010) Diminished neural processing of aversive and rewarding stimuli during selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment. Biol Psychiatry 67:439–445
Menon M, Jensen J, Vitcu I, Graff-Guerrero A, Crawley A, Smith MA, Kapur S (2007) Temporal difference modeling of the blood-oxygen level dependent response during aversive conditioning in humans: effects of dopaminergic modulation. Biol Psychiatry 62:765–72
Meyer-Lindenberg A, Miletich RS, Kohn PD, Esposito G, Carson RE, Quarantelli M, Weinberger DR, Berman KF (2002) Reduced prefrontal activity predicts exaggerated striatal dopaminergic function in schizophrenia. Nat Neurosci 5:267–271
O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C (2001a) Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 4:95–102
O’Doherty J, Rolls ET, Francis S, Bowtell R, McGlone F (2001b) Representation of pleasant and aversive taste in the human brain. J Neurophysiol 85:1315–1321
Palminteri S, Lebreton M, Worbe Y, Grabli D, Hartmann A, Pessiglione M (2009) Pharmacological modulation of subliminal learning in Parkinson’s and Tourette’s syndromes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:19179–19184
Rolls ET, McCabe C (2007) Enhanced affective brain representations of chocolate in cravers vs. non-cravers. Eur J Neurosci 26:1067–1076
Rolls ET, Kringelbach ML, de Araujo IE (2003a) Different representations of pleasant and unpleasant odours in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci 18:695–703
Rolls ET, O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Francis S, Bowtell R, McGlone F (2003b) Representations of pleasant and painful touch in the human orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Cereb Cortex 13:308–317
Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F, Parris BA (2010) Neural systems underlying decisions about affective odors. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1069–1082
Schultz W (2010a) Dopamine signals for reward value and risk: basic and recent data. Behav Brain Funct 6:24
Schultz W (2010b) Multiple functions of dopamine neurons. F1000 Biol Rep 2:2
Small DM, Zatorre RJ, Dagher A, Evans AC, Jones-Gotman M (2001) Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion. Brain 124:1720–1733
Snaith RP, Hamilton M, Morley S, Humayan A, Hargreaves D, Trigwell P (1995) A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale. Br J Psychiatry 167:99–103
Spielberger CD (1983) Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto
Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Gibbon M, First MB (2004) Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV (SCID-I/P). American Psychiatric Press, Washington
Strauss GP, Frank MJ, Waltz JA, Kasanova Z, Herbener ES, Gold JM (2011) Deficits in positive reinforcement learning and uncertainty-driven exploration are associated with distinct aspects of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 69:424–431
Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 54:1063–1070
Wilson JL, Jenkinson M, de Araujo I, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Jezzard P (2002) Fast, fully automated global and local magnetic field optimization for fMRI of the human brain. Neuroimage 17:967–976
Zald DH, Hagen MC, Pardo JV (2002) Neural correlates of tasting concentrated quinine and sugar solutions. J Neurophysiol 87:1068–1075
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the Medical Research Council Grant no (HQRORVO). We would like to acknowledge Mr Leo McCabe for the photography.
Financial disclosures
Dr. McCabe has received consultancy from P1vital Ltd. Dr. Harmer is on the advisory board of P1vital Ltd. and holds shares in the same company. She is also the company director of Oxford Psychologists. Dr. Harmer has received fees for consultancy from Servier, GlaxoSmithKline, Astra Zeneca, Johnson&Johnson, P1vital, Roche and EiSai. Professor Cowen has been a paid member of the advisory boards of Eli Lilly, Servier and Lundbeck and has been a paid lecturer for Eli Lilly, Servier, GlaxoSmithKline. Miss Huber reports no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Open Access
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
Open Access This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0), which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
About this article
Cite this article
McCabe, C., Huber, A., Harmer, C.J. et al. The D2 antagonist sulpiride modulates the neural processing of both rewarding and aversive stimuli in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology 217, 271–278 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2278-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2278-4