Skip to main content
Log in

Bihemifield visual stimulation reveals reduced lateral bias in dyslexia

  • Part II Research Reflections
  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Orton (1936) observed that dyslexic readers display not only obvious linguistic processing errors, but also diminished lateralized specialization of other cerebral hemispheric functions. To explore his “intergrading” hypothesis, six developmental dyslexics (DDs) and a group of good readers (GRs) were tested on measures of interhemispheric coordination. All subjects (ages 16 to 47) demonstrated normal oculomotor control and visual acuity prior to testing. Subjects were instructed to track three different point-light source patterns, (single stimulus in one hemifield, dual stimuli in one hemifield and a pair of simultaneous, symmetric, bihemifield stimuli [SSBS]), presented in random sequence and arrayed horizontally at ±5, ±10, and ±15 degrees eccentricity. Tested with unihemifield stimuli, all subjects showed normal saccadic latencies and trajectories. In response to SSBS, all GRs showed pronounced directional preference, choosing largely to track one side over the other. DDs showed reduced laterality bias (p<.025). DDs showed significantly longer response latencies to SSBS than to unihemifield stimulation (p<.01) and differed significantly from GRs (p<.05).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adler-Grinberg, D. and Stark, L. 1978. Eye movements, scanpaths, and dyslexia.American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics 55:567–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bahill, T. A., Clark, M. R., and Stark, L. 1975. The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements.Math Bioscience 26:191–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breitmeyer, B. G. 1983. Sensory masking, persistence and enhancement in visual exploration and reading.In K. Rayner, (ed.).Eye Movements in Reading: Perceptual and language processes. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, B., Haegerstrom-Portnoy, G., Adams, A. J., Yingling, C. D., Galin, D., Herron, J., and Marcus, M. 1983a. Predictive eye movements do not discriminate between dyslexic and control children.Neuropsychologia 21:121–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, B., Haegerstrom-Portnoy, G., Adams, A. J., Yingling, C. D., Galin, D., Herron, J., and Marcus, M. 1983b. Tracking eye movements are normal in dyslexic children.American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics 60:376–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunt, A. J., Minckler, D. S., and Johanson, G. W. 1977. Demonstration of bilateral projection of the central retina of the monkey with horseradish peroxidase neuronography.Comparative Neurology 171:619–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ciuffreda, K. J., Bahill, A. F., Kenyon, R. V., and Stark, L. 1984. Eye movements during reading: Case reports.American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics 53:389–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, M. 1970.The Dyslexic Child. London: William Heinemann Medical Books Limited.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalby, J. Y. and Gibson, D. 1981. Functional cerebral lateralization in subtypes of disabled readers.Brain and Language 14:34–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, J. D. and Jones, G. M. 1971. Akinesia, hypokinesia, and bradykinesia in the oculomotor system of patients with Parkinson’s disease.Experimental Neurology 32:58–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B. 1979. Childhood learning disabilities.In K. Heilman and E. Valenstein (eds.).Clinical Neuropsychology.

  • Doehring, D. G. 1984. Subtyping of reading disorders: Implications for remediation.Annals of Dyslexia 34:205–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drake, W. E. 1968. Clinical and pathological findings in a child with a developmental learning disability.Journal of Learning Disabilities 1:486–502.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, F. H., Denckla, M. B., Bartels, P. H. and Sandini, G. 1980a. Dyslexia: Regional differences in brain electrical activity by topographic mapping.Annals of Neurology 7:412–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, F. H., Denckla, M. B., Bartels, P. H., Sandini, G. and Kiesslinge, L. S. 1980b. Dyslexia: Automated diagnosis by computerized classification of brain electrical activity.Annals of Neurology 7:421–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elterman, R. D., Abel, L. A., Daroff, R. B., Dell’Osso, L. F. and Bornstein, J. L. 1980. Eye movement patterns in dyslexic children.Journal of Learning Disabilities 13:16–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eskenazi, B. and Diamond, S. P. 1983. Visual Exploration of non-verbal material by dyslexic children.Cortex 19:353–370

    Google Scholar 

  • Findlay, J. M. 1983. Visual information processing for saccadic eye movements.In A. Hein and M. Jeannerod (eds.).Spatially Oriented Behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. H. 1910. A case study of congenital word-blindness (inability to learn to read).Opthalmological Society 30:216–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. M. and Kemper, T. L. 1979. Cytoarchitectonic abnormalities in developmental dyslexia: A case study.Annals of Neurology 6:94–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. M. 1984. Anatomical asymmetries.In N. Geschwind and A. Galaburda (eds.).Cerebral Dominance: The biological foundations. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S. 1974. Cerebral dominance viewed as a decision system.In S. Dimond and J. Beaumont (eds.).Hemisphere Function in the Human Brain. London: Paul Elek.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N. 1982. Why Orton Was Right.Annals of Dyslexia 32:13–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N. and Behan, P. 1982. Left handedness: association with immune disease, migraine and developmental learning disorders.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A. 79:5097–5100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross-Glen, K. and Rothenberg, S. 1984. Evidence for deficit in interhemispheric transfer of information in dyslexic boys.International Journal of Neuroscience 24:23–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hallet, P. E. 1978. Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions.Vision Research 18:1279–1296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heller, D. 1979. Eye movements in dyslexic and normal reading children.Zeitschrift fur Psychologie 187:106–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermann, H. T., Sonnabend, N. L. and Zeevi, Y. Y. 1986. Interhemispheric coordination is compromised in dyslexia.Cortex (in press).

  • Hermann, K. 1959,Reading Disability. Springfield: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinshelwood, J. 1895. Word Blindness and visual memory.Lancet 2:1564–1570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinshelwood, J. 1900.Letter-, Word- and Mind-Blindness. London: H. K. Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinshelwood, J. 1917.Congenital Word-Blindness. London: H. K. Lewis and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, A., and Stark, L., 1980. Reverse staircase reading eye movements.Supp. Invest. Opthal. and Vision Research.

  • Jones, A., and Stark, L. 1983. Abnormal patterns of normal eye movements in specific dyslexia.In K. Rayner (ed.).Eye Movements in Reading (Perceptual and Language Processes). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, G. M. and DeJong, J. D. 1971. Dynamic characteristics of saccadic eye movements in Parkinson’s disease.Experimental Neurology 31:17–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, T. L. 1984. Asymmetrical lesions in dyslexia.In N. Geschwind and A. Galaburda (eds.).Cerebral Dominance: The biological foundations. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenyon, R. V., Becker, J. T., Butters, N. and Hermann, H. T. 1984. Oculomotor functions in Wernicke-Korsakoff’s syndrome: Saccadic eye movements.International Journal of Neurosciences.

  • Kinsbourne, M. and McMurray, J. 1975. The effect of cerebral dominance on time sharing between speaking and tapping by preschool children.Child Development 46:240–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. 1983. Models of learning disability.Topics in Learning and Learning Disabilities 3:1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kris, C. 1969. Developmental and diagnostic EOG and EEG studies of binocular visual responsiveness and oculomotor control in children and adults with perceptual, reading and learning difficulties.Journal of Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 26:633–634.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kussmaul, A. 1877. Disturbance of Speech.In H. von Ziemssen (ed.).Cyclopedia of the practice of medicine 14. 121, translated by J. A. McCreery. New York: William Wood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, B. J., Kupper, D. L., Beggs, V., and Landon, D., 1982. Do learning-disabled children exhibit peripheral deficits in selective attention? An analysis of eye movements during reading.Journal of Child Psychology 10:1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legein, Ch. P., and Bouma, H. 1982. Reading and opthalmologist. An introduction into the complex phenomenon of ordinary reading as a guideline for analysis and treatment of disabled readers.Documenta Opthalmologica 53:123–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leisman, G., and Schwartz, K. J. 1978a. Aetiological factors in dyslexia: 1. Saccadic eye movement control.Perceptual and Motor Skills 47:403–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leisman, G., and Schwartz, J., 1978b. Aetiological factors in dyslexia: II: Ocular-motor programming.Perceptual and Motor Skills 47:667–672.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lévy-Schoen, A. 1969. Determination et latence de la response oculomotrice a deux stimuli.L’Annee Psychologuique 69:373–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, W. P. 1896. A case of congenital wordblindness.British Medical Journal 2:1378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moscovitch, M., and Klein, D. 1980. Material-specific perceptual interference for visual words and faces: Implications for models of capacity limitations, attention and laterality.Journal of Experimental Psychology (Human Perceptual Performance) 6:590–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, F., and Marshall, J. C. 1981. On psycholinguistic classifications of the acquired dyslexias.Bulletin of the Orton Society 31:29–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, F. 1984. Personal communication.

  • Ober, J. and Ùdden, P. 1985. Personal communication.

  • Olson, M. E. 1973. Laterality differences in tachistoscopic word recognition in young normal and delayed readers in elementary school.Neuropsychologia 11:343–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. K., Kliegal, R., and Davidson, B. J. 1983. Dyslexic and normal reader’s eye movements.Experimental Psychology (Human Perception and Performance) 9:816–825.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orton, S. T. 1925. Word Blindness in school children.Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 14:581–615.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orton, S. T. 1936. (Paper presented at the Thomas W. Salmon Memorial Lecture at New York Academy of Medicine.)In S. Orton.Reading, Writing, and Speech Problems in Children. New York: W. W. Norton and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlidis, George Th. 1981. Do eye movements hold the key to dyslexia?Neuropsychologia 19:57–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perlin, H. 1983. Personal communication.

  • Pirozzolo, F. J., and Rayner, K. 1978. Disorders of oculomotor scanning and graphic orientation in developmental Gerstmann syndrome.Brain and Language 5:119–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pirozzolo, F. J. and Hansch, E. C. 1981. Oculomotor time in dementia reflects degree of cerebral dysfunction.Science 214:349–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poynter, H. L., Schor, C., Haynes, H. M., and Hirsch, J. 1982. Oculomotor functions in reading disability.American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics 59:116–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabinovitch, R. D. 1959. Reading and learning disabilities.In S. Arieti (ed.).American Handbook of Psychiatry. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. 1978. Eye movements in reading and information processing.Psychology Bulletin 85:618–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. 1983a. Eye movements, perceptual span and reading disability.Annals of Dyslexia 33:163–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. 1983b. The perceptual span and eye movement control during reading eye movements in reading.In K. Rayner (ed.).Eye Movements in Reading: Perceptual and language processes. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richman, J. E., Walker, A. J., Garzia, R. P. 1983. The impact of automatic digit naming ability on a clinical test of eye movement functioning.Journal of the American Optometric Association 54:617–622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, G. 1978. Eye movements in dyslexic children.In G. V. Stanley and K. W. Walsh (eds.).Brain Impairment: Proceedings of the 1977 Brain Impairment Workshop. Melbourne: Neuropsychology Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, G., Smith, G. A. Howell, E. A. 1983. Eye movements and sequential tracking in dyslexic and control children.British Journal of Psychology 74:181–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, J. F., and Fowler, S. 1982 Diagnosis of dyslexia by means of a new indicator of eye dominance.British Journal of Opthalmolmology 66:332–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temple, C. M., and Marshall, J. C. 1983. A case study of developmental phonological dyslexia.British Journal of Psychology 74:517–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Umilta, C., Rizzolatti, G. Anzola, P., Luppino, G., and Porro, C. 1985. Evidence of interhemispheric transmission in laterality efforts.Neuropsychologia 23:203–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R. 1979.DYSLEXIA: Theory and Research. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiig, E. H. and Semel, E. M. 1976.Language Disabilities in Children and Adolescents. Columbus: Charles E. Merrill Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiig, E. H., and Semel, E. M. 1980.Language Assessment and Intervention for the Learning Disabled. Columbus, Ohio: Charles E. Merrill Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witelson, S. F. 1977. Developmental dyslexia: Two right hemispheres and none left.Science 195:309–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Witelson, S. F. 1985. The brain connection: The corpus callosum is larger in left handers,Science 229:665–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, L. R. 1962.A Sampled Data Model for Eye Tracking Movements. Sc.D. Dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zangwill, O. L. and Blakemore, C. 1972. Dyslexia: Reversal of eye movements during reading.Neuropsychologia 10:371–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeevi, Y. Y., Wetzel, P. and Young L. 1982.Second Interim Technical Progress Report on Visual and Non-visual Factors in Flight Simulation. Contract F336L5-8L-00LL.

  • Zeevi, Y. Y., Wetzel, P., and Young, L. 1983.Temporal Aspects of Eye Movement When Viewing Multiple Targets. AFHRL-TP-83-6, Nov.

  • Zeevi, Y. Y. 1984. Unpublished observations on two left handed subjects.

  • Zeevi, Y. Y., and Hason, A. 1985.Programming of Saccadic Responses to Dual Targets. E.E. Report, Technion-Israel Institute of Technology.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hermann, H.T., Sonnabend, N.L. & Zeevi, Y.Y. Bihemifield visual stimulation reveals reduced lateral bias in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia 36, 154–175 (1986). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02648027

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02648027

Keywords

Navigation