Skip to main content
Log in

Producing simple utterances: Details of the planning process

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Subjects produced subject-verb sentences of prespecified structure, such asThe man is kicking, in response to pictures. The amount of information required to identify the pictured actor or action and to retrieve a name for the actor was varied. The following results, based on subject response latencies, were obtained. Experiment I demonstrated that only some initial amount of verb information processing occurs before and delays the initiation of such subject-verb sentences. Experiment II demonstrated that verb information processing is initiated only after the subject of the sentence has been identified. Finally, Experiment III demonstrated that verb information processing is initiated in parallel with the retrieval of the name for the subject of the sentence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Boomer, D. S. (1965). Hesitation and grammatical encoding.Language Speech 8:148–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brain, R. (1961). The neurology of language.Brain 84:145–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., and McNeill, D. (1966). The “tip of the tongue” phenomenon.J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 5:325–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crossman, E. R. F. W. (1953). Entropy and choice time: The effect of frequency unbalance on choice responses.Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 5:41–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse, P. (1964). Le temps de reaction verbale. I. Denomination et lecture.Ann. Psychol. 64:21–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse, P. (1969). Why is naming longer than reading?Acta Psychol. 30:96–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Eisler, F. G. (1968).Psycholinguistics: Experiments in Spontaneous Speech, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hick, W. E. (1952). On the rate of gain of information.Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 4:11–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyman, R. (1953). Stimulus information as a determinant of RT.J. Exp. Psychol. 45:188–196.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. H. (1967).Biological Foundations of Language, Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, R. K., and Lindsay, J. M. (1966). Reaction time and serial versus parallel information processing.J. Exp. Psychol. 71:294–303.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, J. R. (1975). Producing simple utterances: How far ahead do we plan?Cogn. Psychol. 7:1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1970a). Context-dependent stuttering.Kybernetik 7:1–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1970b). Spoonerisms: The structure of errors in the serial order of speech.Neuropsychologia 8:323–350.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maclay, H., and Osgood, C. E. (1959). Hesitation phenomena in spontaneous English speech.Word 15:19–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. G., and Strange, W. (1968). The perception of hesitation in spontaneous speech.Percept. Psychophys. 3:427–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowbray, G. H. (1960). Choice reaction time for skilled response.Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 12:193–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967).Cognitive Psychology, Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1966). Things, words, and the brain.Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 18:340–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. R. (1970). Language and thought: Aspects of a cognitive theory of semantics.Psychol. Rev. 77:257–273.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E. (1963). On understanding and creating sentences.Am. Psychologist 18:735–751.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, H., Lewis, S. S., and Rubenstein, M. A. (1971). Homographic entries in the internal lexicon: Effects of systematicity and relative frequency of meanings.J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 10:57–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Southworth, F. C. (1967). A model of semantic structure.Language 43:342–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannenbaum, P. H., Williams, F., and Hillier, C. S. (1965). Word predictability in the environments of hesitations.J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 4:134–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L., and Lorge, I. (1944).The Teacher's Wordbook of 30,000 Words, Teacher's College, Columbia University, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wingfield, A. (1968). Effects of frequency on identification and naming of objects.Am. J. Psychol. 81:226–234.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This article is based on a Ph.D. dissertation submitted to New York University.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lindsley, J.R. Producing simple utterances: Details of the planning process. J Psycholinguist Res 5, 331–354 (1976). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01079933

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01079933

Keywords

Navigation