Skip to main content

Learning Individual Verbs and the Verb System: When Are Multiple Examples Helpful?

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Language and Concept Acquisition from Infancy Through Childhood

Abstract

This chapter focuses on the problem of verb learning, including learning the meaning of a single new verb and learning the verb system in a language. Verb learning occurs in three phases: finding the core of meaning, discovering dominant patterns in a language, and delineating boundaries between individual verbs. In the first phase, two types of perceptual similarity are shown to be useful—sound symbolism and object similarity. Children benefit from seeing high-similarity examples before low-similarity ones (progressive alignment), as well as from contrast. After describing how children may discover patterns within a language, we focus on how children learn a verb within an overall system by describing verbs for carrying/holding in Chinese. Children between 3 and 7 years produced fewer verbs than their mothers, better approximated adult verb meanings with age. MDS and INDSCAL analyses reveal they attended to the objects in the events and reveal three semantic islands of verb meaning. An entropy analysis shows that there is an early stage of verb learning in which input frequency is important and a later stage in which the degree of boundary overlap with other verbs affects their ease of acquisition. In sum, the chapter shows children’s use of multiple exemplars for verb learning, using structure mapping as a theoretical framework, and addressing the whole of verb learning in development.

We thank our research labs, the children and families who gave of their time—without whom we would be unable to learn about the world—our departments, and our funding sources (NICHD 2R15 HD044447; MEXT/JSPS 26580078, 16H01928 and 18H05084).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    We had originally conducted separate analyses both for the “carrying” and “holding” matrices, but the results were very similar. Thus, we only report the results of the analysis using the matrix for the “carrying” events to avoid redundancy.

  2. 2.

    In Chinese, the distinction between a morpheme and a word is difficult to make. The 13 verbs were words consisting of a single morpheme. To make sure that the frequency count for each verb does not contain cases in which the same morpheme is a part of a different word (e.g., “ti” [dangle around the arm] used in “ti-gao” [to raise, to improve]), we went through the examples manually and excluded the latter cases from the counts.

References

  • Aichison, J. (1987). Words in the mind: An introduction to the mental lexicon. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ameel, E., Malt, B., & Storms, G. (2008). Object naming and later lexical development: From baby bottle to beer bottle. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 262–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Au, T. K., & Laframboise, D. E. (1990). Acquiring color names via linguistic contrast: The influence of contrasting terms. Child Development, 61, 1808–1823.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Au, T. K., & Markman, E. M. (1987). Acquiring word meanings via linguistic contrast. Cognitive Development, 2, 217–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolinger, D. (1977). Meaning and form. London and New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booth, A. E., & Waxman, S. R. (2009). A horse of a different color: Specifying with precision infants’ mappings of novel nouns and adjectives. Child Development, 80, 15–22.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (1982). Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development. In L. R. Gleitman & E. Wanner (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 319–346). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (2005). Why can’t you “open” a nut or “break a cooked noodle?”: Learning covert object categories in action word meanings. In L. Gershkoff Stowe & D. H. Rakinson (Eds.), Building object categories in developmental time (pp. 209–243). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1978). The child as word learner. In M. Halle, J. Bresnan, & G. A. Miller (Eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality (pp. 264–293). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. D., & Chang, J. J. (1970). Analysis of individual differences in multidimensional scaling via an N-way generalization of “Eckart-Young” decomposition. Psychometrika, 35, 283–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Childers, J. B., Bottera, A., & Howard, T. (2017). Verbs: Learning how speakers use words to refer to actions. In G. Westermann & N. Mani (Eds.), Current issues in developmental psychology: Early word learning (pp. 84–96). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Childers, J. B., Hirshkowitz, A., & Benavides, K. (2014). Attention to explicit and implicit contrast in verb learning. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15, 213–237.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Childers, J. B., & Paik, J. H. (2009). Korean-and English-speaking children use cross-situational information to learn novel predicate terms. Journal of Child Language, 36(1), 201–224.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Childers, J. B., Parrish, R., Olson, C. V., Burch, C., Fung, G., & McIntyre, K. P. (2016). Early verb learning: How do children learn how to compare events? Journal of Cognition and Development, 17(1), 41–66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V. (1990). On the pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language, 17(2), 417–431.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cruse, D. A. (1986). Lexical semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (1995). Modified oral input and the acquisition of word meanings. Applied Linguistics, 16, 409–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, i–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C. J. (1982). Frame semantics. In The Linguistic Society of Korea (Ed.), Linguistics in the morning calm: Selected papers from SICOL 1981 (pp. 111–138). Seoul: Hanshin Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C. J., & Atkins, B. T. (1992). Toward a frame-based lexicon: The semantics of RISK and its neighbors. In A. Lehrer & E. Kittay (Eds.), Frames, fields, and contrasts (pp. 75–102). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C. (1996). Structural limits on verb mapping: The role of analogy in children’s interpretations of sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 31, 41–81.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C. (2002). Structural limits on verb mapping: The role of abstract structure in 2.5-year-olds’ interpretations of novel verbs. Developmental Science, 5, 55–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (1991). On the semantic content of subcategorization frames. Cognitive Psychology, 23(3), 331–392.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning (Center for the Study of Reading, Technical Report No. 257).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. (2010). Bootstrapping the mind: Analogical processes and symbol systems. Cognitive Science, 34(5), 752–775.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2001). Individuation, relativity, and early word learning. Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, 3, 215–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Kurtz, K. J. (2005). Relational categories. In W. K. Ahn, R. L. Goldstone, B. C. Love, A. B. Markman, & P. W. Wolff (Eds.), Categorization inside and outside the lab (pp. 151–175). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., Loewenstein, J., & Hung, B. (2007). Comparison facilitates children’s learning of names for parts. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8(3), 285–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Namy, L. L. (2006). Analogical processes in language learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 297–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Rattermann, M. J. (1991). Language and the career of similarity (Center for the Study of Reading, Technical Report No. 533). University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman, L. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition, 1(1), 3–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., Chung, H. L., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Liu, J., Bertenthal, B. I., Brand, R., et al. (2002). Young children can extend motion verb labels to point-light displays. Developmental Psychology, 38, 604–614.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hao, M., Shu, H., Xing, A., & Li, P. (2008). Early vocabulary inventory for Mandarin Chinese. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 728–733.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haryu, E., & Imai, M. (2002). Reorganizing the lexicon by learning a new word: Japanese children’s interpretation of the meaning of a new word for a familiar artifact. Child Development, 73(5), 1378–1391.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haryu, E., Imai, M., & Okada, H. (2011). Object similarity bootstraps young children to action-based verb extensions. Child Development, 82, 674–686.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollich, G., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2007). Young children associate novel words with complex objects rather than salient parts. Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1051–1061.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., & Gentner, D. (1997). A crosslinguistic study of early word meaning: Universal ontology and linguistic influence. Cognition, 62, 169–200.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., Gentner, D., & Uchida, N. (1994). Children’s theories of word meaning: The role of shape similarity in early acquisition. Cognitive Development, 9, 45–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., & Haryu, E. (2001). How do Japanese children learn proper nouns and common nouns without clues from syntax? Child Development, 72, 787–802.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., Haryu, E., & Okada, H. (2005). Mapping novel nouns and verbs onto dynamic action events: Are verb meanings easier to learn than noun meanings for Japanese children? Child Development, 76(2), 340–355.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., & Kita, S. (2014). The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1651), 20130298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., Li, L., Haryu, E., Okada, H., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., et al. (2008). Novel noun and verb learning in Chinese-, English-, and Japanese-speaking children. Child Development, 79, 979–1000.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kantartzis, K., Imai, M., & Kita, S. (2011). Japanese sound-symbolism facilitates word learning in English-speaking children. Cognitive Science, 35(3), 575–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3(3), 299–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, B. (1993). English verb classes and alternations: A preliminary investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, P., Zhao, X., & MacWhinney, B. (2007). Dynamic self-organization and early lexical development in children. Cognitive Science, 31, 581–612.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lidz, J., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. (2003). Understanding how input matters: Verb learning and the footprint of universal grammar. Cognition, 87, 151–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, J. (1963). Structural semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics (Vol. 1). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (1987). The competition model. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 249–308). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maguire, M. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Brandone, A. C. (2008). Focusing on the relation: Fewer exemplars facilitate children’s initial verb learning and extension. Developmental Science, 11, 628–634.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization in children: Problems of induction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M. (1990). Constraints children place on word meanings. Cognitive Science, 14(1), 57–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M., & Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children’s sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic vs. thematic relations. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 121–157.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G., & Johnson-Laird, P. (1976). Language and perception. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mori, T., & Yoshida, T. (1990). Technical book of data analysis for psychology. Kyoto: Kitaoji Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muehleisen, V., & Imai, M. (1997). Transitivity and the incorporation of ground information in Japanese path verbs. In K. Lee, E. Sweetwer, & M. Verspoor (Eds.), Lexical and syntactic constructions and the construction of meaning (pp. 329–346). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naigles, L. (1990). Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. Journal of Child Language, 17, 357–374.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Namy, L. L., & Clepper, L. E. (2010). The differing roles of comparison and contrast in children’s categorization. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 291–305.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perniss, P., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). The bridge of iconicity: From a world of experience to the experience of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pustejovsky, J. (1995). The generative lexicon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Parish-Morris, J., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2009). Live action: Can young children learn verbs from video? Child Development, 80, 1360–1375.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Saji, N., Imai, M., Saalbach, H., Zhang, Y., Shu, H., & Okada, H. (2011). Word learning does not end at fast-mapping: Evolution of verb meanings through reorganization of an entire semantic domain. Cognition, 118(1), 45–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saussure, F. de (1916/1983). Course in general linguistics (R. Harris, Trans). London: Duckworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaefer, R. P. (1979). Child and adult verb categories. Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics, 4, 61–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, R. M., & Fisher, C. (2009). Two-year-olds use distributional cues to interpret transitivity-alternating verbs. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(6), 777–803.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1975). Syntax and semantics of motion. Syntax and Semantics, 4, 181–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1985). Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description (pp. 36–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition, 74, 209–253.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. E. (1994). Learning words in nonostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30(5), 639–650.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Kruger, A. C. (1992). Joint attention on actions: Acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts. Journal of Child Language, 19(2), 311–333.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S. R., & Klibanoff, R. S. (2000). The role of comparison in the extension of novel adjectives. Developmental Psychology, 36, 571–581.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S. R., Lidz, J. L., Braun, I. E., & Lavin, T. (2009). Twenty four-month-old infants’ interpretations of novel verbs and nouns in dynamic scenes. Cognitive Psychology, 59, 67–95.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yoshida, H. (2012). A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism in children’s verb learning. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 232–265.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, S., & Fisher, C. (2009). “Really? She blicked the baby?”: Two-year-olds learn combinatorial facts about verbs by listening. Psychological Science, 20, 619–626.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mutsumi Imai .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Imai, M., Childers, J.B. (2020). Learning Individual Verbs and the Verb System: When Are Multiple Examples Helpful?. In: Childers, J. (eds) Language and Concept Acquisition from Infancy Through Childhood. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-35594-4_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics