Skip to main content
Log in

Exploring the boundaries of the framing effect: The moderating roles of disparate expected values and perceived costs of judgmental errors

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Framing effects on retail store choice decisions were investigated in four experiments. Subjects preferred the store that guaranteed (a “sure” option) good prices (experiment 1), product availability (experiment 2), or a rebate (experiment 3) when consequences were framed in terms of gains; subjects preferred the risky option when consequences were framed in terms of losses. Consistent with fuzzy-trace theory, framing effects were reduced when the expected values of options were disparate in a direction that disfavored sure gain or probabilistic loss options (experiment 2) and when the perceived costs of committing a judgmental error were high (experiment 3). Experiment 4 shows that the moderating effects of disparate expected values and costs of judgment errors generalize to within-subject designs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arkes, Hal R. (1991). “Costs and Benefits of Judgment Errors: Implications for Debiasing,”Psychological Bulletin 110, 486–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettman, James R., and Mita Sujan. (1987). “Effect of Framing on Evaluation of Comparable and Noncomparable Alternatives by Experts and Novice Consumers,”Journal of Consumer Research 14, 141–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischhoff, Baruch. (1983). “Predicting Frames,”Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 9, 103–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammond, Kenneth R., Robert M. Hamm, Janet Grassia, and Tamra Pearson. (1987). “Direct Comparison of the Efficacy of Intuitive and Analytical Cognition in Expert Judgment,”IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics SMC-17, 753–770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Josephs, Robert A., Richard P. Larrick, Claude M. Steele, and Richard E. Nisbett. (1992). “Protecting the Self from the Negative Consequences of Risky Decisions,”Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 62, 26–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky. (1979). “Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision Making Under Risk,”Econometrica 47, 263–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky. (1984). “Choices, Values, and Frames,”American Psychologist 39, 341–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, Arie W. (1989).Lay Epistemics and Human Knowledge. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, Irwin P., and Gary J. Gaeth. (1988). “How Consumers Are Affected by the Framing of Attribute Information Before and After Consuming the Product,”Journal of Consumer Research 15, 374–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maheswaran, Durairaj, and Joan Meyers-Levy. (1990). “The Influence of Message Framing and Issue Involvement,”Journal of Marketing Research 27, 361–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeil, Barbara J., Stephen J. Pauker, Harold C. Sox, Jr., and Amos Tversky. (1982). “On the Elicitation of Preferences for Alternative Therapies,”New England Journal of Medicine 306, 1259–1262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John W., James R. Bettman, and Eric J. Johnson. (1992). “Behavioral Decision Research: A Constructive Perspective,”Annual Review of Psychology 43, 87–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puto, Christopher P. (1987). “The Framing of Buying Decisions,”Journal of Consumer Research 14, 301–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Qualls, William J., and Christopher P. Puto. (1989). “Organizational Climate and Decision Framing: An Integrated Approach to Analyzing Industrial Buying Decisions,”Journal of Marketing Research 26, 179–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, Valerie F., and Charles J. Brainerd. (1991). “Fuzzy-Trace Theory and Framing Effects in Choice: Gist Extraction, Truncation, and Conversion,”Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 4, 249–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, Valerie F., and Charles J. Brainerd. (1992). “A Fuzzy-Trace Theory of Reasoning and Remembering: Paradoxes, Patterns, and Parallelism,” in Alice F. Healy, Stephen M. Kosslyn, and Richard M. Shiffrin (eds.),From Learning Processes to Cognitive Processes: Essays in Honor of William K. Estes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonson, Itamar. (1992). “The Influence of Anticipating Regret and Responsibility on Purchase Decisions,”Journal of Consumer Research 19, 105–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, Richard H. (1989). “Toward a Positive Theory of Consumer Choice,”Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 1, 39–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, Richard H. (1985). “Mental Accounting and Consumer Choice,”Marketing Science 4, 199–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, Richard H. (1987). “The Psychology of Choice and the Assumptions of Economics,” in Alvin Roth (ed.),Laboratory Experiments in Economics: Six Points of View. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, Richard H., and Eric J. Johnson. (1990). “Gambling with the House Money and Trying to Break Even: The Effects of Prior Outcomes on Risky Choice,”Management Science 36, 643–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. (1981). “The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice,”Science 211, 453–458.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kellaris, J.J., Kardes, F.R. & Dinovo, T. Exploring the boundaries of the framing effect: The moderating roles of disparate expected values and perceived costs of judgmental errors. Marketing Letters 6, 175–182 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00995109

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00995109

Key words

Navigation