Skip to main content
Log in

Do Affective States Influence Risk Preferences?

Evidence from Incentive-Compatible Experiments

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Schmalenbach Business Review Aims and scope

Abstract

Recent discussions in decision sciences and economics stress the potential impact of affect on decision outcomes. In this study, we conducted incentive-compatible laboratory experiments (N = 253) to investigate whether affect causes temporary fluctuations in risk preferences. In particular, we employed film clips to induce participants into joyful, fearful and sad affective states and subsequently elicited risk preferences by asking the participants to make choices among different lotteries. The financial consequences of the lottery choices varied randomly among the fixed-, low-, and high-stakes treatment groups. We find only weak evidence that affective states influence risk preferences. In particular, we find some evidence that sadness leads to risk aversion, but we find no effects for joy and fear. Our findings question recent claims in the literature that the relationship between affect and risk preferences is strong and unambiguous.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Affect is often employed as an umbrella term that refers to the current moods and emotions of individuals. Moods are generally described as low-intensity, diffuse, and relatively enduring affective states that often arise for no particularly salient reason. Compared with moods, emotions are considered to be more intense and short-lived affective states that generally have a definite cause and clear cognitive content (Davidson 1994; Gray and Watson 2001). The distinctions between moods and emotions are largely theoretical rather than empirical in nature. In research practice, identical methods are often used to induce both moods and emotions (Fredrickson 2002). In this paper, we use the terms “affect”, “affective state”, and “mood” interchangeably. Nevertheless, we believe that the definition of mood – low-intensity, diffuse, and not object-directed – is more appropriate to our empirical examination than the definition of emotion – intense, object-directed, and with clear cognitive content.

  2. Pham (2007) presents a critical review on the empirical evidence on the relationship between emotion and rationality.

  3. According to Thomson Reuter’s web of science, 188 articles have been published on the relationship between affect (emotion, mood) and risk references in the social sciences since 1980 until 2014.

  4. The Journal of Business & Psychology edited a special issue addressing null results and several articles in this special issue served as excellent examples when null results can be meaningful.

  5. Overall, the internal reliabilities for extroversion (Cronbach’s α = 0.74), agreeableness (α = 0.23), conscientiousness (α = 0.71), emotional stability (α = 0.69), and openness (α = 0.52) were not satisfactory. Therefore, we conducted a rotated factor analysis and extracted four components with eigenvalues that were greater than 1, that is, extroversion, emotional stability, conscientiousness, and openness. Agreeableness was not found to be a separate factor and was not included as a control variable in the analysis. The principal component analysis (PCA) considers all available information, whereas sum scores of the items for one personality dimension would ignore the personality differences among participants. The factor scores and the sum scores were nearly perfectly correlated for four of the five personality dimensions examined (rs > 0.90, ps < 0.001).

  6. We pre-tested film clips from “Silence of the Lambs” and “Halloween” as suggested by Hewig et al. (2005) to induce fear in a sample of 50 students. These film clips did not significantly induce a fearful state in our participants. Thus, we chose to use a film clip from “Paranormal Activity” that successfully induced the desired fearful state.

  7. We used factor scores for the analysis because they consider all the available information, whereas sum scores of the items for one mood type would ignore the mood differences among participants that arose from changes in fear or sadness. We included the same PANAS-X items in the principal component analysis (PCA) factor scores as in the sum scores. Varimax rotation is applied. The factor scores and sum scores were nearly perfectly correlated for all three of the examined moods (r > 0.95, p < 0.001). The results are robust to using sum scores instead of factor scores.

  8. d = 0.20 denotes a small effect, d = 0.50 a medium effect, and d = 0.80 a large effect (Cohen 1988).

  9. Detailed correlation results available upon request.

  10. No evidence against poolability was produced by a poolability analysis across the factor of gender.

  11. We only consider α = 0.05 given that none of our results survives the conservative Bonferroni correction. Statistical power was calculated using formulas derived from first principals, which yields results that are identical to those obtained from using standard software packages such as GPOWER (Erdfelder et al. 1996).

  12. Ibid.

References

  • Allen, N.B., and P.B.T. Badcock. 2003. The social risk hypothesis of depressed mood: evolutionary, psychosocial, and neurobiological perspectives. Psychological Bulletin 129:887–913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, S., G.W. Harrison, M.I. Lau, and E.E. Rutstroem. 2006. Elicitation using multiple price list formats. Experimental Economics 9:383–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C.J. 2003. The psychology of doing nothing: forms of decision avoidance result from reason and emotion. Psychological Bulletin 129:139–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashenfelter, O., C. Harmon, and H. Oosterbeek. 1999. A review of estimates of the schooling/earnings relationship, with tests for publication bias. Labour Economics 6:453–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baillon, A., P.D. Koellinger, and T. Treffers. 2014. Sadder but wiser: the effects of affective states and weather on ambiguity attitudes. http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2418946. Accessed 1 December 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blavatskyy, P.R. 2009. Betting on own knowledge: experimental test of overconfidence. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 38:39–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botzen, W.J.W., J. de Boer, and T. Terpstra. 2013. Framing of risk and preferences for annual and multi-year flood insurance. Journal of Economic Psychology 39:357–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Callen, M., M. Isaqzadeh, J.D. Long, and C. Sprenger. 2014. Violence and risk preference: experimental evidence from Afghanistan. American Economic Review 104:123–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C.F., and R.M. Hogarth. 1999. The effects of financial incentives in experiments: a review and capital-labor-production framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 19:7–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, L., and M. Shah. 2012. Risk-taking behavior in the wake of natural disasters. http://www.econstor.eu/bitstream/10419/62540/1/721216366.pdf. Accessed 1 December 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C.S., and E. Harmon-Jones. 2009. Anger is an approach-related affect: evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin 135:183–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiles, T.H., and J.F. McMackin. 1996. Integrating variable risk preferences, trust, and transaction cost theory. Academy of Management Review 21:73–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chow, G.C. 1960. Tests of equality between sets of coefficients in two linear regressions. Econometrica 28:591–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, A., J. Engelmann, E. Fehr, and M.A. Maréchal. 2015. Evidence for countercyclical risk aversion: an experiment with financial professionals. American Economic Review 105(2):860–885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conte, A., M.V. Levati, and C. Nardi. 2013. Risk preferences and the role of emotions. https://papers.econ.mpg.de/esi/discussionpapers/2013-046.pdf. Accessed 1 December 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  • Croson, R., and U. Gneezy. 2009. Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature 47:448–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. 1994. On emotion, mood, and related affective constructs. In The nature of emotion: fundamental questions, ed. P. Ekman, R. Davidson, 51–55. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickersin, K., and Y.I. Min. 1993. Publication bias: the problem that won’t go away. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 703:135–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dohmen, T., A. Falk, D. Huffman, U. Sunde, J. Schupp, and G.G. Wagner. 2011. Individual risk attitudes: measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association 9:522–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C.C., M.A. El-Gamal, and R.K. Wilson. 2009. Risk loving after the storm: a Bayesian-network study of Hurricane Katrina evacuees. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 69:110–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. 1992. An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion 6:169–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erdfelder, E., F. Faul, and A. Buchner. 1996. GPOWER: a general power analysis program. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, & Computer 28:1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr-Duda, H., T. Epper, A. Bruhin, and R. Schubert. 2011. Risk and rationality: the effects of mood and decision rules on probability weighting. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 78:14–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finucane, M.L., A. Alhakami, P. Slovic, and S.M. Johnson. 2000. The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 13:1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbacher, U. 2007. z‑Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics 10:171–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, P., T. Greitemeyer, A. Kastenmüller, C. Vogrincic, and A. Sauer. 2011. The effects of risk-glorifying media exposure on risk-positive cognitions, emotions, and behaviors: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 137:367–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J.P. 1995. Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin 117:39–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. 2002. Positive emotions. In Handbook of positive psychology, ed. C. Snyder, S. Lopez, 120–134. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerrards-Hesse, A., K. Spies, and F.W. Hesse. 1994. Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: a review. British Journal of Psychology 85:55–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goeree, J.K., C.A. Holt, and T.R. Palfrey. 2003. Risk averse behavior in generalized matching pennies games. Games & Economic Behavior 45:97–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Göritz, A.S., and K. Moser. 2006. Web-based mood induction. Cognition & Emotion 20:887–896.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, S.D., P.J. Rentfrow, and W.B. Swann Jr.. 2003. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality 37:504–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grable, J.E., and M.J. Roszkowski. 2008. The influence of mood on the willingness to take financial risks. Journal of Risk Research 11:905–923.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, E., and D. Watson. 2001. Emotion, mood, and temperament: similarities, differences and a synthesis. In Emotions at work: theory, research and applications in management, ed. R. Payne, C. Cooper, 21–34. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greiner, B. 2004. The online recruitment system ORSEE – a guide for the organization of experiments in economics. In Forschung und wissenschaftliches Rechnen 2003. GWDG Bericht 63, ed. K. Kremer, V. Macho, 79–93. Göttingen: Gesellschaft für Wissenschaftliche Datenverarbeitung. Research and scientific computation 2003. GWDG report 63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J.J., and R.W. Levenson. 1995. Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition & Emotion 9:87–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guiso, L., P. Sapienza, and L. Zingales. 2013. Time varying risk aversion. http://www.kellogg.northwestern.edu/faculty/sapienza/htm/risk_aversion.pdf. Accessed 1 December 2015.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gujarati, D. 1970. Use of dummy variables in testing for equality between sets of coefficients in linear regressions: a generalization. American Statistician 24:18–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E. 2003. Anger and the behavioral approach system. Personality & Individual Differences 35:995–1005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, G.W., M.I. Lau, and E.E. Rutstroem. 2009. Risk attitudes, randomization to treatment, and self-selection into experiments. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 70:498–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haushofer, J., and E. Fehr. 2014. On the psychology of poverty. Science 344:862.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, R.M., L.G. Crişan, D. Houser, M. Miclea, and A.C. Miu. 2010. Emotion regulation and decision making under risk and uncertainty. Emotion 10:257–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewig, J., D. Hagemann, J. Seifert, M. Gollwitzer, E. Naumann, and D. Bartussek. 2005. A revised film set for the induction of basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion 19:1095–1109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hey, J. 2001. Does repetition improve consistency? Experimental Economics 4:5–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirshleifer, D., and T. Shumway. 2003. Good day sunshine: stock returns and the weather. Journal of Finance 58:1009–1032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hockey, G.R.J., A.J. Maule, P.J. Clough, and L. Bdzola. 2000. Effects of negative mood states on risk in everyday decision making. Cognition & Emotion 14:823–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holt, C.A., and S.K. Laury. 2002. Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review 92:1644–1655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ifcher, J., and H. Zarghamee. 2011. Happiness and time preference: the effect of positive affect in a random-assignment experiment. American Economic Review 101:3109–3129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ioannidis, J.P. 2005. Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Medicine 2:e124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ioannidis, J.P. 2008. Why most discovered true associations are inflated. Epidemiology 19(5):640–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A.M. 2000. Positive affect and decision making. In Handbook of emotion, ed. M. Lewis, J.M. Haviland-Jones, 417–435. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A.M., and R. Patrick. 1983. The effect of positive feelings on risk taking: when the chips are down. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 31:194–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson-Stenman, O. 2010. Risk aversion and expected utility of consumption over time. Games & Economic Behavior 68:208–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamstra, M.J., L.A. Kramer, and M.D. Levi. 2000. Losing sleep at the market: the daylight saving anomaly. American Economic Review 90:1005–1011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamstra, M.J., L.A. Kramer, and M.D. Levi. 2003. Winter blues: a SAD stock market cycle. American Economic Review 93:324–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, M.Y., and R. Kanfer. 2009. The joint influence of mood and a cognitively demanding task on risk-taking. Motivation & Emotion 33:362–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kliger, D., and O. Levy. 2003. Mood-induced variation in risk preferences. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 52:573–584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kugler, T., T. Connolly, and L.A. Ordóñez. 2012. Emotion, decision, and risk: betting on gambles versus betting on people. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 25:123–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landis, R.S., L.R. James, C.E. Lance, C.A. Pierce, and S.G. Rogelberg. 2014. When is nothing something? Editorial for the null results special issue of journal of business and psychology. Journal of Business & Psychology 29:163–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leith, K.P., and R.F. Baumeister. 1996. Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk tasking, and self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71:1250–1267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J.S., and D. Keltner. 2000. Beyond valence: toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition & Emotion 14:473–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J.S., and D. Keltner. 2001. Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81:146–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J.S., D.A. Small, and G. Loewenstein. 2004. Heart strings and purse strings carryover effects of emotions on economic decisions. Psychological Science 15:337–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, O., and I. Galili. 2008. Stock purchase and the weather: individual differences. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 67:755–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, Y., and J. Yagil. 2011. Air pollution and stock returns in the US. Journal of Economic Psychology 32:374–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, S., T. Odean, and E.B. Andrade. 2012. Bubbling with excitement: an experiment. http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2024549. Accessed 1 December 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G.F., E.U. Weber, C.K. Hsee, and N. Welch. 2001. Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin 127:267–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mukherjee, K. 2010. A dual system model of preferences under risk. Psychological Review 177:243–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Open Science Collaboration. 2015. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349(6251):aac4716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pham, M.T. 2004. The logic of feeling. Journal of Consumer Psychology 14:360–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pham, M.T. 2007. Emotion and rationality: a critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Review of General Psychology 11:155–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raghunathan, R., and M.T. Pham. 1999. All negative moods are not equal: motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 79:56–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, C.A., et al, 2014. Common genetic variants associated with cognitive performance identified using the proxy-phenotype method. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111(38):13790–13794.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders Jr., E.M. 1993. Stock prices and Wall Street weather. American Economic Review 83:1337–1345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K.R. 2005. What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information 44:695–729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., and G.L. Clore. 1983. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45:513–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H.C. 2010. Investor mood and financial markets. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 76:267–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Silva, D.G., R.A.J. Pownall, and L. Wolk. 2012. Does the sun ‘shine’on art prices? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 82:167–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, V.L. 1982. Microeconomic systems as an experimental science. American Economic Review 72:923–955.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, A.L., S.B. Kirk, C.L. Cameron, and S. Danoff-Burg. 2000. Coping through emotional approach: scale construction and validation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78:1150–1169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, G.J., and G.S. Becker. 1977. De gustibus non est disputandum. American Economic Review 67:76–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sunstein, C.R. 2003. Terrorism and probability neglect. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 26:121–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verheyen, C., and A.S. Göritz. 2009. Plain texts as an online mood-induction procedure. Social Psychology 40:6–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viscusi, W.K. 2009. Valuing risks of death from terrorism and natural disasters. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 38:191–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voors, M.J., E.E.M. Nillesen, P. Verwimp, E.H. Bulte, R. Lensink, and D.P. Van Soest. 2012. Violent conflict and behavior: a field experiment in Burundi. American Economic Review 102:941–964.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., and L.A. Clark. 1994. The PANAS-X: manual for the positive and negative affect schedule – expanded form. Iowa City: University of Iowa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., L.A. Clark, and A. Tellegen. 1988. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54:1063–1070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, E.U., A. Blais, and N.E. Betz. 2002. A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 15:263–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegener, D.T., and R.E. Petty. 1994. Mood management across affective states: the hedonic contingency hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66:1034–1048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westermann, R., K. Spies, G. Stahl, and F.W. Hesse. 1996. Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: a meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology 26:557–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, K., L. Zheng, and Q. Zhu. 2006. Are investors moonstruck? Lunar phases and stock returns. Journal of Empirical Finance 13:1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Theresa Treffers.

Additional information

We thank the Erasmus Research Institute of Management (ERIM) and the German Research Foundation (PI 97 /15-1) for financial and organizational support.

Appendix

Appendix

We estimated regressions using the model specifications in Table 4 and only the incentive-compatible low- and high-financial-stakes observations from our sample in studies 1 and 2 (N = 148). There are no significant effects of moods on risk preferences after correction for multiple testing.

Table 6 OLS regressions on risk preferences with incentive-compatible observations

In Table 7, we used the excluded observations from study 1 (N = 188) and calculated regressions with identical model specifications as for models 1 and 2 in Table 4. These observations were obtained from participants in study 1 who had completed one or two other tasks between the completion of their mood induction and their risk-preference assessment. Previous studies (Kim and Kanfer 2009) have demonstrated that mood induction becomes weaker as the time period between the induction and task measurement becomes longer. It is therefore unsurprising that none of the mood treatments had a significant effect on risk preferences in the data from these excluded observations. Indeed, none of the experimental treatments had a significant effect in these data after correcting for multiple testing.

Table 7 OLS regressions on risk preferences with the observations excluded from study 1

In Table 8, we used the pooled data from study 1 (N = 278) and conducted regression analyses with the model specifications of models 1 and 2 in Table 4. We find no significant mood effects on risk preferences.

Table 8 OLS regressions on risk preferences with pooled observations from study 1

Table 9 reports the regression results by financial stakes when using the pooled data from study 1 (N = 278) and model specifications identical to those in Table 5. None of the experimental treatments has a significant effect in these data after correcting for multiple testing.

Table 9 OLS regressions on risk preferences by stakes with observations from study 1

In Table 10, we used the pooled high-stakes observations from study 1 and from study 2 (N = 92) to conduct a regression analysis with model specifications identical to those in Model 1 in Table 4. We do not find significant effects of moods on risk preferences.

Table 10 OLS regressions on risk preferences with pooled high-stakes observations from studies 1 and 2

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Treffers, T., Koellinger, P.D. & Picot, A. Do Affective States Influence Risk Preferences?. Schmalenbach Bus Rev 17, 309–335 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s41464-016-0018-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41464-016-0018-3

Keywords

JEL-Classification

Navigation