Skip to main content
Log in

6- and 8-year-olds’ performance evaluations: Do they differ between self and unknown others?

  • Published:
Metacognition and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The current study investigated kindergarteners and second graders’ ability to monitor and evaluate their own and a virtual peer’s performance in a paired-associate learning task. Participants provided confidence judgments (CJs) for their own responses and performance-based judgments (judgments provided after receiving feedback on their performance) for both their own and a virtual peer’s responses. For the performance-based judgments, children were confronted with their own or the peer’s answer as well as the correct answer. Additionally, participants were asked to credit their own and the peer’s correct and incorrect answers while facing feedback. Results indicate an age-related progression in metacognitive monitoring skills, with second graders differentiating more strongly in their confidence judgments between correct and incorrect responses compared to kindergarteners. Regarding performance-based judgments, children of both age groups provided higher judgments for correctly compared to incorrectly recognized items as well as for their own responses in comparison to the responses of the unknown child. Similarly, when crediting, participants of both age groups gave more credits for correct recognition than for incorrect recognition and for their own responses than for the peer’s responses. The significant interaction between age group and recognition accuracy for the crediting shows that second graders gave more credits for correctly recognized items while kindergarteners gave more credits for incorrect answers than the older children – primarily for their own incorrect answers. In conclusion, the study provides new insights into 6- and 8-year-olds’ evaluations of their own and an unknown child’s performance in a paired-associate learning task by showing that children of both age groups generally judged and credited responses in their own favor. These results add to our understanding of biases in children’s performance evaluations, including metacognitive judgments and judgments provided after receiving feedback.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215 Retrieved from https://www.uky.edu/~eushe2/Bandura/Bandura1977PR.pdf.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernard, S., Clément, F., & Mercier, H. (2016). Wishful thinking in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 267–274. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L. (1978). Knowing when, where, and how to remember: A problem of metacognition. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (pp. 77–165). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Destan, N., & Roebers, C. M. (2015). What are the metacognitive costs of young children’s overconfidence? Metacognition and Learning, 10, 347–374. doi:10.1007/s11409-014-9133-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Destan, N., Hembacher, E., Ghetti, S., & Roebers, C. M. (2014). Early metacognitive abilities: The interplay of monitoring and control processes in 5- to 7-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 213–228. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.1004.1001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognition. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2012). Overconfidence produces underachievement: Inaccurate self evaluations undermine students' learning and retention. Learning and Instruction, 22, 271–280. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.08.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D. (2005). Self-insight: Roadblocks and detours on the path to knowing thyself. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1979). Meta-cognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.34.10.906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, D. J., Bol, L., Horgan, D. D., & Rakow, E. A. (2000). Test prediction and performance in a classroom context. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 160–170. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.92.1.160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1984). Emergent achievement behavior: Some early developments. In J. Nicholls (Ed.), Advances in achievement motivation (pp. 1–32). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., Hines, J. C., & Touron, D. R. (2013). Judgments of learning are influenced by multiple cues in addition to memory for past test accuracy. Archives of Scientific Psychology, 1, 23–32. doi:10.1037/arc0000003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howie, P., & Roebers, C. M. (2007). Developmental progression in the confidence-accuracy relationship in event recall: Insights provided by a calibration perspective. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 871–893. doi:10.1002/acp.1302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juslin, P., Winman, A., & Olsson, H. (2000). Naive empiricism and dogmatism in confidence research: A critical examination of the hard-easy effect. Psychological Review, 107, 384–396. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.107.2.384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one's own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 126, 349–370. doi:10.1037//0096-3445.126.4.349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Nussinson, R., Bless, H., & Shaked, N. (2008). Information-based and experience-based metacognitive judgments: Evidence from subjective confidence. In J. Dunlosky & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Handbook of metamemory and memory (pp. 117–136). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Ackerman, R., Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). The memorizing effort heuristic in judgments of learning: A developmental perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 265–279. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.10.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, S. S., & Roebers, C. M. (2012). The impact of retrieval processes, age, general achievement level, and test scoring scheme for children's metacognitive monitoring and controlling. Metacognition and Learning, 7, 75–90. doi:10.1007/s11409-011-9079-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, J. (1999). Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 221–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R. (2007). Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 317–344. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenstein, S., & Fischhoff, B. (1977). Do those who know more also know more about how much they know? Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 20, 159–183. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(77)90001-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipko, A. R., Dunlosky, J., Hartwig, M. K., Rawson, K. A., Swan, K., & Cook, D. (2009). Using standards to improve middle school students' accuracy at evaluating the quality of their recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 307–318. doi:10.1037/a0017599.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipko, A. R., Dunlosky, J., Lipowski, S. L., & Merriman, W. E. (2012). Young children are not underconfident with practice: The benefit of ignoring a fallible memory heuristic. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 174–188. doi:10.1080/15248372.2011.577760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, K. E., & Ghetti, S. (2011). The development of uncertainty monitoring in early childhood. Child Development, 82, 1178–1787. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01649.x 21954871.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, K. E., & Ghetti, S. (2013). I don't want to pick! Introspection on uncertainty supports early strategic behavior. Child Development, 84, 726–736. doi:10.1111/cdev.12004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 125–173). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J. G. (1978). Development of concepts of effort and ability, perception of academic attainment, and understanding that difficult tasks require more ability. Child Development, 49, 800–814. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1978.tb02383.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Okita, S. Y. (2014). Learning from the folly of others: Learning to self-correct by monitoring the reasoning of virtual characters in a computer-supported mathematics learning environment. Computers & Education, 71, 257–278. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, M., Tsalas, N., Proust, J., & Sodian, B. (2014). Metacognitive monitoring of oneself and others: Developmental changes during childhood and adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 153–165. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.12.011 24607803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1930). The child's conception of physical causality. London, UK: Routledge & Keagan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plumert, J. M., & Schwebel, D. C. (1997). Social and temperamental influences on children's overestimation of their physical abilities: Links to accidental injuries. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 317–337. doi:10.1006/jecp.1997.2411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posey, E., & Smith, R. A. (2003). The self-serving bias in children. Psi Chi Journal of Undergraduate Research, 8, 153–156 Retrieved from http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/homepage/class/Psy359H/Echols/Psi%20Chi%20Articles/2003%20Vol%208/2003%20Vol%208%20%234/Posey.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roderer, T., & Roebers, C. M. (2010). Explicit and implicit confidence judgments and developmental differences in metamemory: An eye-tracking approach. Metacognition and Learning, 5, 229–250. doi:10.1007/s11409-010-9059-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M. (2002). Confidence judgments in children's and adults' event recall and suggestibility. Developmental Psychology, 38, 1052–1067. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.38.6.1052.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M. (2014). Children's deliberate memory development: The contribution of strategies and metacognitive processes. In P. J. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley Handbook on the development of Children's memory (pp. 865–894). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., & Spiess, M. A. (2016). The development of metacognitive monitoring and control in second graders: A short-term longitudinal study. Journal of Cognition and Development. doi:10.1080/15248372.2016.1157079.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., Krebs, S. S., & Roderer, T. (2014). Metacognitive monitoring and control in elementary school children: Their interrelations and their role for test performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 29, 141–149. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.12.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (1998). Performance prediction in young children: Effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking. Developmental Science, 1, 291–297. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (2014). Memory development from early childhood through emerging adulthood. Heidelberg, DE: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, S. L., & Laurion, S. K. (1993). Do we know what we've learned from listening to the news? Memory & Cognition, 21(2), 198–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Lockl, K. (2002). The development of metacognitive knowledge in children and adolescents. In T. J. Perfect & B. L. Schwartz (Eds.), Applied metacognition (pp. 224–257). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Lockl, K. (2008). Procedural metacognition in children: Evidence for developmental trends. In J. Dunlosky & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Handbook of metamemory and memory (pp. 391–409). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Visé, M., Lockl, K., & Nelson, T. O. (2000). Developmental trends in children's memory monitoring - evidence from a judgment-of-learning task. Cognitive Development, 15, 115–134. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00024-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schunk, D. H. (1983). Ability versus effort attributional feedback: Differential effects on self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(6), 848–856 Retrieved from https://libres.uncg.edu/ir/uncg/f/D_Schunk_Ability_1983.pdf.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint actions: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Science, 10, 70–76. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M. L., Stephan, W. G., & Rosenfield, D. (1978). Attributional egotism. In J. H. Harvey, W. J. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 2, pp. 91–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sodian, B. (1990). Understanding sources of information - implications for early strategy use. Interactions among Aptitudes, Strategies, and Knowledge in Cognitive Performance, 12–21 Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-1-4612-3268-1_2.

  • Stipek, D. (1984). Young children's performance expectations: Logical analysis or wishful thinking. In J. Nicholls (Ed.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 3, pp. 33–56). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D. J., & Hoffman, J. M. (1980). Development of children’s performanceerelated judgments. Child Development, 51, 912–914. doi:10.2307/1129485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D. J., & Tannatt, L. M. (1984). Childrens judgments of their own and their peers academic competence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 75–84. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.76.1.75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsalas, N., Paulus, M., & Sodian, B. (2015). Developmental changes and the effect of self-generated feedback in metacognitive controlled spacing strategies in 7-year-olds, 10-year-olds, and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 140–154. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.01.008 25703006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Loon, M. H., de Bruin, A. B. H., van Gog, T., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2013). The effect of delayed-JOLs and sentence generation on children's monitoring accuracy and regulation of idiom study. Metacognition and Learning, 8, 173–191. doi:10.1007/s11409-013-9100-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visé, M., & Schneider, W. (2000). Determinanten der Leistungsvorhersage bei Kindergarten- und Grundschulkindern: Zur Bedeutung metakognitiver und motivationaler Einflußfaktoren. Zeitschrift fuer Entwicklungspsychologie und Paedagogische Psychologie, 32, 51–58. doi:10.1026//0049-8637.32.2.51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M. (1985). The origins of metacognition. In D. L. Forrest-Pressley, G. E. MacKinnon, & T. G. Waller (Eds.), Metacognition, cognition, and human performance (Vol. 1, pp. 1–31). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participating children and their families, the children’s teachers and the student research assistants who helped with data collection. Many thanks also to Jakob Raible for his valuable help in task design and programming.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nesrin Destan.

Ethics declarations

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Conflict of interest

This project was partially financed by a grant of the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF- Grant No.100014_126559 provided to the author Claudia M. Roebers. The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Destan, N., Spiess, M.A., de Bruin, A. et al. 6- and 8-year-olds’ performance evaluations: Do they differ between self and unknown others?. Metacognition Learning 12, 315–336 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-017-9170-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-017-9170-5

Keywords

Navigation