Skip to main content
Log in

The role of socioeconomic status in Chinese word reading and writing among Chinese kindergarten children

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The number of Chinese children living in poverty has risen steadily in Hong Kong, China. However, little is known on the longitudinal effects of family socioeconomic status (SES) on cognitive-linguistic skills, word reading and writing in children from low-SES backgrounds. This study examines differences in cognitive-linguistic skills and word reading and writing across kindergarteners from the low- and middle-SES groups. It also investigates the direct link between SES and children’s concurrent Chinese literacy skills and how cognitive-linguistic skills mediate the longitudinal associations between SES and word reading and writing. One hundred and nine children were assessed at Kindergarten K2 (Time 1) and reassessed at Kindergarten K3 (Time 2). Children were administered the measures of vocabulary knowledge, phonological awareness, morphological awareness, executive functioning, Chinese word reading, and Chinese word dictation at both time points. Results revealed that low-SES children performed less well on Chinese word reading and dictation than their middle-SES peers at both time points. Moreover, hierarchical regression and mediation analyses suggested that SES had a direct relation with Chinese word reading at Time 1 after controlling for age and cognitive-linguistic skills, and an indirect link with Chinese word dictation at Time 2 which was fully mediated by phonological awareness and vocabulary knowledge at Time 1. The results highlight that SES may exert an impact on children’s early Chinese reading and writing and suggest the benefit of improving phonological awareness and vocabulary knowledge of low-SES children.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bowey, J. A. (1995). Socioeconomic status differences in preschool phonological sensitivity and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology,87(3), 476–487. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.87.3.476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cabell, S. Q., Justice, L. M., Logan, J. R., & Konold, T. R. (2013). Emergent literacy profiles among prekindergarten children from low-SES backgrounds: Longitudinal considerations. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,28(3), 608–620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.03.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189–209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, Y., & Wu, X. (2017). The relationship between SES and reading comprehension in Chinese: A mediation model. Frontiers in Psychology,8, 672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chow, B. W., Ho, C. S. H., Wong, S. L., Waye, M. Y., & Zheng, M. (2017). Home environmental influences on children’s language and reading skills in a genetically sensitive design: Are socioeconomic status and home literacy environment environmental mediators and moderators? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,58(6), 519–529. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. K. H. (2015). Socioeconomic Status and Academic Achievement. In J. D. Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (2nd ed., Vol. 22, pp. 924–930). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. K. H., Lam, C. B., & Cheung, K. C. (2018). Visuomotor integration and executive functioning are uniquely linked to Chinese word reading and writing in kindergarten children. Reading and Writing,31(1), 155–171. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9779-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. K. H., Liu, H., McBride, C., Wong, A. M., & Lo, J. M. (2017). How socioeconomic status, executive functioning and verbal interactions contribute to early academic achievement in Chinese children. Educational Psychology,37(4), 402–420. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2016.1179264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. K. H., & McBride-Chang, C. (2011). Executive functioning skills uniquely predict Chinese word reading. Journal of Educational Psychology,103(4), 909–921. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Education Bureau of HKSARG. (2006). Guide to the pre-primary curriculum. Retrieved from http://www.edb.gov.hk/attachment/en/curriculum-development/major-level-of-edu/preprimary/pre-primaryguide-net_en.pdf. Accessed 10 Aug 2018.

  • Hassunah Arafat, S., Korat, O., Aram, D., & Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2017). Continuity in literacy achievements from kindergarten to first grade: A longitudinal study of Arabic-speaking children. Reading and Writing,30(5), 989–1007. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9709-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hecht, S. A., Burgess, S. R., Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2000). Explaining social class differences in growth of reading skills from beginning kindergarten through fourth-grade: The role of phonological awareness, rate of access, and print knowledge. Reading and Writing,12(1–2), 99–127. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008033824385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S. H., Chan, D. W., Chung, K. K. H., Tsang, S. M., Lee, S. H., & Cheng, R. W. Y. (2007). The Hong Kong test of specific learning difficulties in reading and writing for primary school students (HKT-P(II)) (2nd ed.). Hong Kong: Hong Kong Specific Learning Difficulties Research Team.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hong Kong Education Department and Hong Kong Psychological Society. (1981). HK-WISC manual: Hong Kong-Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Hong Kong, China: Hong Kong Government Printer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horton-lkard, R., & Weismer, S. E. (2007). A preliminary examination of vocabulary and word learning in African American toddlers from middle and low socioeconomic status homes. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,16(4), 381–392. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2007/041).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • International Labour Organization. (2008). ISCO international standard classification of occupations. Retrieved from http://www.ilo.org/public/english/bureau/stat/isco/isco08/index.htm. Accessed 10 Aug 2018.

  • Lam, S. S., & McBride, C. (2018). Learning to write: The role of handwriting for Chinese spelling in kindergarten children. Journal of Educational Psychology,110(7), 917–930. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, B. S. (Ed.). (1999). New Chinese dictionary. Hong Kong: China Book.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, D., Chung, K. K. H., & McBride, C. (2016). The role of SES in Chinese (L1) and English (L2) word reading in Chinese-speaking kindergarteners. Journal of Research in Reading,39(3), 268–291. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12046.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Y., Sun, H., Lin, D., Li, H., Yeung, S. S., & Wong, T. T. (2018). The unique role of executive function skills in predicting Hong Kong kindergarteners’ reading comprehension. British Journal of Educational Psychology,88(4), 628–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Larsman, P., & Strid, A. (2012). Development of phonological awareness during the preschool year: The influence of gender and socio-economic status. Reading and Writing,25(2), 305–320. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9269-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. S. H. (2000). Developmental issues in Chinese children’s character acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology,92(1), 50–55. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Wanless, S. B., & Murray, A. (2007). Executive function, behavioral self-regulation, and social-emotional competence: Links to school readiness. In O. N. Saracho & B. Spodek (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on social learning in early childhood education (pp. 83–107). Charlotte, NC: Information Age.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist,53(2), 185–204. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyers, L. S., Gamst, G., & Guarino, A. J. (2006). Applied multivariate research: Design and interpretation. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology,98(1), 134–147. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, K. G., Farah, M. J., & McCandliss, B. D. (2006a). Socioeconomic background modulates cognition-achievement relationships in reading. Cognitive Development,21(3), 349–368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.01.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, K. G., Wolmetz, M. E., Ochs, L. G., Farah, M. J., & McCandliss, B. D. (2006b). Brain-behavior relationships in reading acquisition are modulated by socioeconomic factors. Developmental Science,9(6), 642–654. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00542.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pallante, D. H., & Kim, Y. (2013). The effect of a multicomponent literacy instruction model on literacy growth for kindergartners and first-grade students in Chile. International Journal of Psychology,48(5), 747–761. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.719628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, J., Kong, Y., Song, S., McBride, C., Liu, H., & Shu, H. (2017). Socioeconomic status, parent report of children’s early language skills, and late literacy skills: A long term follow-up study among Chinese children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,30(2), 401–416. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9682-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puglisi, M. L., Hulme, C., Hamilton, L. G., & Snowling, M. J. (2017). The home literacy environment is a correlate, but perhaps not a cause, of variations in children’s language and literacy development. Scientific Studies of Reading,21(6), 498–514. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1346660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raag, T., Kusiak, K., Tumilty, M., Kelemen, A., Bernheimer, H., & Bond, J. (2011). Reconsidering SES and gender divides in literacy achievement: Are the gaps across social class and gender necessary? Educational Psychology,31(6), 691–705. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2011.599835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, M. E., & Fish, M. (2010). Language skills in low-SES rural Appalachian children: Kindergarten to middle childhood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,31(3), 238–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2010.02.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sohr-Preston, S. L., Scaramella, L. V., Martin, M. J., Neppl, T. K., Ontai, L., & Conger, R. (2013). Parental socioeconomic status, communication, and children’s vocabulary development: A third-generation test of the family investment model. Child Development,84(3), 1046–1062. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Su, M., Peyre, H., Song, S., Mcbride, C., Tardif, T., Li, H., et al. (2017). The influence of early linguistic skills and family factors on literacy acquisition in Chinese children: Follow-up from age 3 to age 11. Learning and Instruction,49, 54–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.12.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Lan, X., Son, S., Cameron, C. E., Morrison, F. J., et al. (2013). Gender differences in behavioral regulation in four societies: The United States, Taiwan, South Korea, and China. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,28(3), 621–633. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.04.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yan, C. W., McBride-Chang, C., Wagner, R. K., Zhang, J., Wong, A. Y., & Shu, H. (2012). Writing quality in Chinese children: Speed and fluency matter. Reading and Writing,25(7), 1499–1521. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9330-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, X., Peng, P., & Meng, X. (2019). Contributions of basic cognitive processing to Chinese reading: The mediation effect of basic language processing. Frontiers in Psychology,9, 2670. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Y., Tardif, T., Shu, H., Li, H., Liu, H., McBride-Chang, C., et al. (2013). Phonological skills and vocabulary knowledge mediate socioeconomic status effects in predicting reading outcomes for Chinese children. Developmental Psychology,49(4), 665–671. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhuang, Z. Y. (2000). 香港小學課本用字規範 [Standard characters used in Hong Kong primary school textbooks]. Hong Kong: Joint.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Research Grants Council of the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region, China (GRF projects HKIED 841212 and 840308) to Chung Kevin Kien Hoa.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kevin Kien Hoa Chung.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fung, Wk., Chung, K.K. The role of socioeconomic status in Chinese word reading and writing among Chinese kindergarten children. Read Writ 33, 377–397 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09967-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09967-2

Keywords

Navigation