In the training of laparoscopic surgery residents require more training compared to “open” surgery to successfully refine their sensorimotor system [1] due to the limitations of 2D vision (on the flat video display panel), difficult hand–eye coordination in the small 3D working space and the use of counterintuitive instruments with distorted tactile feedback.

Despite rapid technological advancements in the simulators including addition of force tracking and visual or haptic feedback, learning of a specific laparoscopic task is still very demanding and time-consuming. Because of work hour restrictions in the EU, UK and USA there is a pressing need for new models and techniques to make training more efficient and the learning curve for laparoscopic operations steeper [2, 3].

Our hypothesis, which has not been investigated previously, was that by allowing trainees to haptically explore an object before executing a laparoscopic action on that object, the action can be performed better and safer (i.e. with lower force parameter outcomes), can be performed quicker (i.e. shorter task time) or more efficient (less path length).

Therefore, a study was performed in which students executed basic laparoscopic tasks in a box trainer with or without a pre-test haptical exploration of the object on which they had to do the “surgical” action.

Methods

An open randomized controlled trial. Experiment was conducted at the Amsterdam University Medical Centre, location VU Medical Centre.

For an overview of the study design, see Fig. 1. Participants were randomized between two groups using block randomization (with random block sizes between 4 and 8) by using www.randomization.com. This resulted in equal allocation in both groups.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Graphical representation of the design of the study. Twenty students were divided in either a group of 10 that was allowed to touch the model or a group of 10 that was only allowed to see the model

Subjects

Subjects were recruited using a social media post, posters inside the medical faculty and direct approach form researchers. To be eligible for participation they had to meet the following criteria: medical student, no visual or haptic handicaps (e.g. colour blindness, neuropathy). Additional information such as gender, age, previous laparoscopic experience, years in medical school and gaming habits was gathered from all subjects. All participants signed an informed consent form. The study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines for experimental investigation with human subjects of the University Medical Centres Amsterdam. Participation in the study did not have any consequences for their academic progress. All participants who enrolled in the study completed the experiment.

Tasks

Task 1 (see Fig. 1), ‘Post and Sleeve’ (3Dmed, Franklin, Ohio, USA) is a previously validated laparoscopic task performed with two curved Maryland dissection forceps [4]. Task 2 (see Fig. 1 and video 1) ‘Experimental task’ was created especially for this experiment to simulate tissue manipulation. It uses a silicone ileocecal model (Simsei© Appendectomy model, Applied Medical, Rancho Santa Margarita, California, USA). Besides being very life-like in shape it was chosen especially because of its difference in the “tissue” consistency of- and between the “small bowel”, “large bowel”, “Appendix” and “meso-appendix”. For further description of the tasks we refer to Appendix.

Study design (see Fig. 1)

In order to get used to the equipment and compare baseline laparoscopic performance, both groups started the experiment by performing three repetitions of a standard laparoscopic “post and sleeve” task (Task 1) on a box trainer. Beforehand, they watched video-instructions on how to perform task 1. Subsequently, all participants watched the video-instructions for task 2: a task on a silicone life-like anatomic ileocecal model. Group A (Touch group) was instructed to take the anatomic model in their hands and to explore the geometry and different structures of the model with their hands (Fig. 2) for the duration of two minutes. The subjects in Group B (No-Touch group) were not allowed to touch the model but were instructed to attentively look at the model from all angles for a period of 2 min without restrictions on the distance between face and task. Thereafter, both groups performed ten repetitions of task 2. The performance of the first and the last repetition was compared between groups. The total time needed for the experiment was 40 min per participant.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Haptic exploration of the silicone ileocecal model. The circles and numbers indicate the place and the order in which a needle should be driven through the structures

Materials and measurements

Both tasks were performed on the LAPSTAR box trainer (Camtronics, Son en Breugel, The Netherlands) (Fig. 3). The box was equipped with the ForceSense measuring system (MediShield, Delft, The Netherlands). This system provides force-, motion- and time parameter outcomes used for objective assessment of laparoscopic performance [5]. It consists of a decoupled three degrees of freedom (DF) force sensor measuring force exerted on the task by the laparoscopic instruments. Furthermore, it registers three DF movements of the instruments and the time needed to complete the task. Based on their proven discrimination power, the following seven parameters were analysed: time needed to complete the task, total distance travelled by the tips of the instruments (path length), mean force, maximal force, maximal impulse, standard deviation of force and force volume. For a more detailed description of the parameters used, see Table 1. In addition, video recordings were captured of each trial.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The LAPSTAR box trainer (Camtronics, Son en Breugel, The Netherlands) equipped with the ForceSense sensor and the hard- and software package that allows for tissue interaction force and 3D instrument motion measurements

Table 1 Description of parameters

Analysis

For data analysis, IBM SPSS Statistics version 22.0 was used with non-parametric tests as the data were not normally distributed. To compare baseline characteristics between group A and B in the pre-test on task 1, the non-parametric Mann–Whitney U test was used. The same test was used to compare the different performance parameter outcomes for task 2 between groups in the final trials. Differences were considered statistically significant if p < 0.05 (tested double-sided).

Results

Participants

As all subjects were able to finalize the different elements of the experiment in time, a total of 20 subjects were included in the analysis. Baseline characteristics are shown in Table 2. The two groups were comparable since no significant differences were found in age, sex or gaming experience. In addition, all participants did have little to no laparoscopic experience on a training system and no experience in laparoscopic surgery.

Table 2 Baseline characteristics

Baseline laparoscopic performance

Baseline laparoscopic performance of the participants was assessed using task 1, ‘Post and sleeve’. During the first two trials, participants familiarized themselves with the laparoscopic equipment. Analysis of the third trial showed no significant differences in time, motion or force parameters (Table 3).

Table 3 Baseline performance (Task 1: peg transfer)

Performance on experimental task

Figure 4 shows boxplots of the seven different performance parameters for each repetition. One can see from these figures that there is no difference for time and path length for both groups. This was statistically confirmed although for path length a trend seems to be present towards less path length used in the “Touch” group. Looking at all four force parameters, values are lower and ranges are smaller in the Touch group. Statistical analysis values show a significant reduction for all force parameters (Table 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Graphic representation of median and standard deviation outcomes of different parameters for the 10 repetitions of the experimental task 2. In blue the no-touch group and in green the touch group

Table 4 Performance experimental task (Task 2)

Discussion

Similar as in airline and air force pilot training, a number of surgical simulators have been designed and validated for laparoscopic skills training. Psychomotor skill learning can be achieved with simulation, reaching learning curves in a more expedite fashion [6]. Simulation training has also been proven to decrease error rates and improve patient care and performance in the OR [7,8,9].

Alternative mental training techniques have been investigated as an adjunct to physical surgical skills training (e.g. systematically and repeatedly imagining an object movement without actually performing it) but so far, for surgical training, the results have been variable [10, 11].

In learning a surgical technical task it seems clinically very relevant that not only the interaction between hands and laparoscopic instruments is important but also the effect this interaction has on the object on which it is performed. Good and solid neurosensory information about the object could therefore be advantageous in order to avoid tissue damage or blood flow reduction. Everyday visual object recognition is not a passive task but involves active exploration using other organs, principally the hands. Besides the help they provide for visual recognition (by manipulating the object it can be viewed from more angles) the hands, through their ability to feel shape, texture, consistency, vibration, temperature and other tissue properties provide tactile sensory information. Together with kinaesthetic perception from receptors in muscles, tendons and joints which provide position, movement and force information this leads to the haptic perception in our brain [12].There is increasing evidence in the neuropsychological literature that visual and haptic information combine or converge to form a mental representation of an object which, because of the combination of these inputs, is more refined and robust than the representation formed by only one of these sensory inputs [13,14,15,16]. Haptic information is defined as the combination of sensory input through the tactile receptors in the skin and the kinesthesic receptors in muscles, tendons and joints. Its value is paramount in open surgery, where “feeling” the tissues is as important as visualizing them [17]. Only a few studies have been conducted comparing haptic exploration versus exploration with instruments on an object in a minimally invasive surgery setting, but the results are invariably in favour of the haptic exploration group [18, 19].

Our novel hypothesis was that by allowing trainees to physically explore an object before executing a laparoscopic action on that object, the action can be performed safer (i.e. lower force parameter outcomes), more efficient (less path length) and can be performed more quickly (i.e. shorter task time). The results indicated that this hypothesis holds for the force parameters that represent safe tissue-handling. We postulated that the “old fashioned” haptic interaction with the patients’ living and healthy, diseased or altered tissue contributes to the “mental image” and “memory” of the tissue, organ or body part that surgeons develop. Especially of how to handle, interpret or predict the reaction of a tissue, organ or body part while manipulating it during an operation. Our findings suggest that it seems likely that in training residents for a particular (laparoscopic) skill, attentive haptic exploration of the object on which the skill has to be learned, could lead to better performance of the skill. Conducting an experiment in a “top down” approach using fMRI imaging or extensive neuropsychological testing seemed too complicated to attain relevant results with the relatively small experimental groups we had at our disposal. Therefore we chose the more surgical “bottom up” approach where we compared the visual only- to the visual combined with haptic exploration of an experimental skill training object. Our silicone model was, by expert surgeons, experienced as very life-like in shape but especially in its difference in the “tissue” consistency between the “small bowel”, “large bowel”, appendix” and “meso-appendix”. Because of this haptic difference and the clinically relevant shape it proved to be a good model to test the effect of haptic exploration.

The most striking finding in our study is that significant less force is used in the group who had been able to haptically explore the experimental object before performing the action on that object. This can be seen in all force measurements as indicated in Fig. 3 and Table 4.

The use of less force has been previously validated as being a good indicator of proper, more respectful and safer tissue-handling [20, 21]. The use of less force is, in other studies, also a recurring indicator of more experience in performing the action or more experience with tissue and instrument handling as seen in experts compared to novices [22]. One can interpret this as that the action is performed more subtle and with more “feeling”, indicating better anticipation to the tissue properties. Other studies show that the same kind of difference in force reduction is seen when haptic feedback is added to instruments in a virtual reality laparoscopic skill training action [22]. So, the reduction in force measured in our study in the “haptic” group can be interpreted either as an indicator of more sensitivity (or a lower threshold) to haptic feedback through the laparoscopic instruments and/or of an already existing experience with the tissue properties stored in the memory of our subjects as a result of the haptic exploration of the object.

The results from our study show that the amount of time or amount of movement, as indicated by total path length travelled by the instruments, was not significantly different, although there is an indication of a trend towards less path length as can be seen in Fig. 3. A post-hoc power analysis (samplesizepwr.m, Mathworks, Natick, MA), based on the task time and path length data indicates a trend towards a significant reduction of path length and Task time if 111 and 749 participants are included. There are however studies that show that the amount of time for an action to be completed is not an adequate indicator of technical competence in performing or learning a task for novices [23]. So the lack of significant difference between our groups for these parameter outcomes does seem less relevant.

The outcomes of this study are relevant in two ways. First it shows that surgical trainees can profit from haptic exploration of 3D models of complex organs before surgery and secondary it indicates the need for more sensitive instruments that allow for accurate haptic feedback.

More research is needed to clarify the exact neuropsychological explanation of our positive results but the effect is significant. Therefore haptic exploration of an object before doing a laparoscopic action on that object seems to be a relevant addition to laparoscopic skills training.

Limitations of this study are that it was performed using medical students and not surgical residents, which, we think, could have led to other and probably even more accentuated differences. Further studies should include an inventory of hobbies/sports that potentially influenced the tactile haptic maturity of an individual. What could also be of interest is to see the progression of trainees that start with different parameter levels, so a study could be conducted that includes correlations between pre and post measurements of individual trainees. Also the use of non-viable tissue models could be of limitation because of the more crude differences in the silicone models compared to human organs. And for clinical implications, the differences in haptic perception of an organ between human individuals, because of differences in sex, build, amount of fat and age, but also whether an organ is inflamed or contains malignancy can severely alter the way it “feels”, so a haptic pre-operative exploration on a “standard” ex-vitro model could limit the clinical results which is, of course also the disadvantage of training on “no touch” laparoscopic models.

In the near future we will conduct new experiments with larger group sizes based on our findings in this study and try to further prove our hypothesis.

Conclusion

The results of this study suggest that trainees can benefit from haptic exploration on a representative model before conducting a laparoscopic action. The object is treated more gently as a result of the haptic exploration and it can therefore be a relevant addition to laparoscopic skills training.