Introduction

Olfactory function plays a key role in human life [1] regulating food ingestion, emotional responses, social and reproductive behavior [2]. Olfactory function decreases over the lifespan [3,4,5] and approximately 5% of the general population exhibits functional anosmia [6]. People with olfactory disorders showed increased risk in food poisoning and cooking, or heating gas injuries due to their inability to identify spoiled food or to detect a gas leak [2, 7, 8]. For instance, participants with olfactory deficits showed impairments in food intake, social life, cognitive function, and personal hygiene with a negative impact in daily life [2, 9,10,11]. Loss of olfactory function is closely linked to both mood and affective disorders in younger and older adults [12]. On average, olfactory function in humans changes not only in relation to age [3,4,5], but also in relation to sex [13,14,15,16], cultural differences in olfactory experience [17, 18], genetic factors [19], infections [20], head trauma [12], and neurodegenerative diseases [21,22,23,24], or emotional disorders [25].

Olfactory function in humans is often evaluated by odor threshold which partly reflects the anatomy of the nasal cavity [26], the expression of olfactory receptors in the nasal epithelium [27] and the olfactory bulb volume [28, 29]. In contrast, odor identification and discrimination are more strongly associated with cultural differences and involve cognitive functions to a relatively larger degree [30]. In addition, a positive correlation was found between cultural and typical food odors, pleasantness, and identification of a smell [31]. Hence, because of these experience-dependent factors, odor identification tests have been adapted to different countries in order to account for these cultural/regional differences [32,33,34,35,36,37].

In Italy, to our knowledge, only few studies investigated olfactory function in healthy subjects. In particular, Eibenstein and colleagues in 2005 [38], through the Sniffin’ Sticks Screening test (Identification test with 12 items), assessed the familiarity and identification of the 12 odors in Italian normosmic subjects, while the Maremmani’s study [39] assessed the validity and reliability of the Italian Olfactory Identification Test (IOIT) [38, 39]. Later, Cantone and colleagues evaluated regional differences in the odor hedonic perception of an odor, but only in the cities of Padua, Rome, Naples and Siracusa, using the Sniffin’ Sticks Identification test (16 items) [40].

Still, up to now, the exact prevalence of olfactory dysfunction in Italy remains unknown, and particularly its relation to different variables of interest, such as age, sex, cognitive ability, cognitive reserve, and level of depression has not fully explored.

Considering these findings, we aimed to perform a more comprehensive investigation of olfactory function on a wider sample of Italian subjects, focusing on the prevalence of olfactory dysfunction in different Italian regions representative of the North, Centre and South of Italy. In addition, we related these data with age, sex, cognitive ability, cognitive reserve index, and depression level.

Methods

Participants

A total of 633 participants (347 women and 286 men) were recruited from 10 different Italian regions (mean age 44.9 years, SD 17.3, age range 18–86, Fig. 1), namely: Sardinia (N = 93), Trentino (N = 56), Lazio (N = 39), Campania (N = 52), Tuscany (N = 94), Piedmont (N = 61), Friuli-Venezia Giulia (N = 57), Sicily (N = 40), Emilia-Romagna (N = 52), Veneto (N = 89). Participants were recruited using a convenience sapling and were divided into six different age groups: 18–29 years (N = 157), 30–39 years (N = 129), 40–49 years (N = 99), 50–59 years (N = 106), > 60 (N = 142). The study was conducted simultaneously in the 10 Italian regions from September 2018 to December 2019 in pre-COVID-19 pandemic. The exclusion criteria were the following: history of neurologic disease (such as epilepsy, brain tumor, Parkinson's disease, Alzheimer disease), major head injury, local respiratory tract factors such as active rhinitis or sinusitis (allergic or infectious) at the moment of the testing, and any cancer or treatment for cancer (chemotherapy or head or neck radiation).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Sample distribution in the Italian regions involved in the study

The demographic/clinical interview for each participant included age, sex, employment, weight (kg), height (cm), body mass index (BMI, calculated as ratio of height and weight, expressed as kg/m2), current medications, and smoking history.

Ethical standard

This study was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki on Biomedical Studies for human subjects. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants. The study design was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University Hospital of Cagliari (Prot. Number: NP/2018/1630).

Procedures

Olfactory function, depression, cognitive reserve, and cognitive function were evaluated, respectively, as follows: “Sniffin’ Sticks” 16-item odor Identification (OI) test (Burghart, Wedel, Germany) [41,42,43], Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), Cognitive Reserve Index (CRI) [44], Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [45]. All questionnaires and examinations were performed for each participant in one session in a well-ventilated room with little or no background odor. Exclusion criteria were age < 18 years, otolaryngology disorders, severe renal dysfunction, neurodegenerative disorders or other comorbidities influencing olfactory function, as well as dementia and psychiatric conditions interfering with the study participation.

Olfactory function was assessed with a standardized and reliable OI (Burghart, Wedel, Germany) which consists of 16 common odors presented together with four verbal descriptors in a multiple forced-choice format (three distractors and one target). Updated normative data reported a score in OI test was ≥ 12 correct answers [43].

The cognitive status was screened through the Italian version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), which measures cognitive abilities in different domains: attention and concentration, executive functions, memory, language, visual-constructional skills, conceptual thinking, calculations, and spatial orientation [45]. The total possible score of the MoCA test was 30 and any score ≥ 26 was considered normal.

In addition, the cognitive reserve was quantified by using the “Cognitive Reserve Index (CRI)” [44]. This self-reported questionnaire quantifies the amount of cognitive reserve acquired during a person’s lifetime. CRI conveys three main sources: education, working activity, and leisure time activities. Each of these features (items) of an individual’s lifetime is recorded as a sub-score.

The depression was evaluated by means of the Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI II) [46], which is a self-reported questionnaire with 21 items examining how participants have been feeling during the last two weeks. Each item was rated in increasing severity from 0 to 3. The overall depression level was classified as minimal (= 0–13), mild (= 14–19), moderate (= 20–28), and severe depression (= 29–63) according to the sum of the item ratings. The total experimental procedure required approximately 75–90 min.

Statistical analysis

A sample size calculation was performed to assess the required minimum number of subjects to be enrolled in the study. Based on previous studies using similar protocols [5, 21, 47, 48], a minimum number of about 450 total subjects was considered adequate to detect investigated differences. In fact, a power calculation, considering a critical effect size f = 0.20–0.25 (medium effect), with 95% power and a 5% significance level in a standard two-way ANOVA, suggested a minimal required number of about 450 total subjects, and a power calculation considering a critical effect size f2 = 0.10–0.15 (medium effect), with 95% power, and a 5% significance level for each investigated factor in a multiple linear regression model, suggested a required minimum number of about 200 total subjects.

Data are presented as mean values ± standard deviation (SD). At first, between-subject one-way ANOVA with sex, age, BDI, MoCA, and CRI as covariates was performed to assess differences in the OI due to the region of origin for each participant. The effect of age and gender on OI, MoCA, CRI, and BDI was assessed by separate two-way ANOVAs. For significant effects from ANOVAs, multiple pairwise comparisons were performed with the Tukey’s (HSD) test in the case of significant interactions, or with Bonferroni’s corrected pairwise t tests in all the other cases.

In addition, effect size estimations (Cohen’s d for any significant pairwise comparison) were also reported in the Results section where appropriate (a value of 0.2, 0.5 or 0.8 indicates a small, medium or large effect size, respectively).

To identify the more promising factors for the multivariate regression analyses, bivariate correlations between the OI versus sex, age, MoCA, CRI, and BDI scores were assessed using Pearson’s correlations (r). Furthermore, an exploratory stepwise multivariate linear regression analysis was performed to assess the potential contribution of each significant correlated factor (such as age, sex, MoCA, CRI, and BDI score) on OI. This stepwise method allowed us to evaluate the predictive power of each independent variable in a series of incremental models excluding the no significant ones. In the multivariate linear regression analysis, the OI scores were set as the dependent variable, while age, sex, and CRI scores were independent variables (predictors). In order to perform the multivariate linear regression analysis using a stepwise selection. In model 1, were calculated the correlation between OI score with the independent variable, age, then in model 2, were included the age and CRI. Finally, in model 3, were added the age, the CRI score, and sex. Statistical analyses performed to assess the sample size were carried out using the software GPower 3.1, while all other statistical analyses were carried out by the SPSS software version 22 for Windows (IBM, Armonk, N.Y., USA). The significance level was set at p < 0.05.

Results

Odor identification

The total sample was n = 633 (347 women and 286 men) with an age that ranged between 18 and 86 years. Descriptive statistics were performed to establish age-related normative values based on the OI test in Italian population (Table 1). In addition, descriptive characteristics of the sample for each Italian region are provided in Table 2. In our sample (Table 3), around 27% (n = 172) of participants showed an OI score lower than 12, thus indicating hyposmia [5, 43]. In detail (Table 3), the regions Tuscany and Emilia-Romagna showed a high frequency (around 50%) of olfactory deficits, whereas Sardinia, Trentino, Piedmont, and Friuli-Venezia Giulia exhibited a low frequency (around 15%) of OI impairment.

Table 1 Normative values for the Odor identification (OI) Sniffin’ Sticks test scores
Table 2 Descriptive statistics for age in the total sample and for each Italian region
Table 3 Percentage of olfactory deficits (OI score < 12) for each Italian region

Differences between Italian regions

In Fig. 2 mean ± SD of OI score in the ten Italian regions were reported. We found a significant effect of the region factor [F(9, 633) = 10.11, p < 0.0001, partial η2 = 0.127], that was still present after controlling for covariates such as sex, age, MoCA, CRI, and BDI [F(9, 633) = 6.51, p < 0.0001, partial η2 = 0.087]. The Tukey’s HSD post hoc test revealed that scores found in Sardinia, the highest ones (means ± SD = 13.43 ± 2.12), were significantly higher than those in Lazio (means ± SD = 11.95 ± 2.17, p = 0.023, Cohen’s d = 0.69), Campania (means ± SD = 11.98 ± 2.07, p = 0.009, Cohen’s d = 0.69), Emilia-Romagna (means ± SD = 11.13 ± 1.97, p < 0.0001, Cohen’s d = 1.12) and Tuscany, the lower ones (means ± SD = 11.00 ± 3.53, p < 0.0001, Cohen’s d = 0.83). Similar results to Sardinia were found for Trentino (means ± SD = 13.05 ± 1.56) and Friuli-Venezia Giulia (means ± SD = 13.10 ± 1.73), with Piedmont (means ± SD = 12.72 ± 2.14), Sicily (means ± SD = 12.47 ± 2.02), and Veneto (means ± SD = 12.93 ± 1.81) displaying intermediate values.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Means ± SD of olfactory identification (OI) in the ten Italian regions. One-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s multiple pairwise comparisons. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ****p < 0.0001 versus Sardinia (the region with the higher OI score); #p < 0.05, ####p < 0.0001 versus Tuscany (the region with the lower OI score)

Effects of age and sex on Odor identification, cognitive ability, cognitive reserve index and depression

As shown in Fig. 3A, both factors, the age group and sex, had a significant effect on OI scores [age group: F(4, 633) = 23.50, p < 0.0001, partial η2 = 0.131; sex: F(1, 633) = 4.65, p = 0.032, partial η2 = 0.007] but not their interaction [age group × sex: F(4, 633) = 1.29, p = 0.273, partial η2 = 0.008]. Bonferroni’s pairwise comparisons revealed a significant reduction of the olfactory identification in the older age group (> 60) compared to all other age groups, in both men and women (from p < 0.001 to p < 0.0001; Cohen’s d = 0.87 for men 18–29 vs > 60; Cohen’s d = 1.05 for women 18–29 vs > 60) (Fig. 3A for single points of statistical significance).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Role of sex and age on the olfactory identification (OI) (A), cognitive abilities (MoCA) (B), cognitive reserve index (CRI) (C) and depression level (BDI) (D), respectively. Data are indicated as means ± SD. Two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s or Tukey’s multiple pairwise comparisons. *p < 0.05; ***p < 0.001; ****p < 0.0001 versus age group > 60 women; ΔΔp < 0.01; ΔΔΔp < 0.001; ΔΔΔΔp < 0.0001 versus age group > 60 men; #p < 0.05; ###p < 0.001; ####p < 0.0001 versus age group 18–29 women; θθθp < 0.001; θθθθp < 0.0001 versus age group > 60 men; §§p < 0.01 women versus men

Two-way ANOVA also detected a significant effect for the age group [age group: F(4, 633) = 16.46, p < 0.0001, partial η2 = 0.096], but not for the sex [sex: F(1, 633) = 0.12, p = 0.733, partial η2 = 0.000] nor a significant interaction [age group × sex: F(4, 633) = 0.83, p = 0.988, partial η2 = 0.000] in the MoCA test. Similarly, to data emerged for the OI, a significant age-dependent cognitive decline was observed both in men and women and along the age groups. Accordingly, in Bonferroni’s pairwise comparisons were found significant differences between the younger age groups (i.e., groups from 18–29 to 40–49) and the older ones (> 60) in both sexes (from p < 0.05 to p < 0.0001; Cohen’s d = 0.78 for men 18–29 vs > 60; Cohen’s d = 0.66 for women 18–29 vs > 60) (Fig. 3B for single statistical significances).

In the same vein, the age group [age group: F(4, 633) = 22.23, p < 0.0001, partial η2 = 0.125], but not sex [sex: F(1, 633) = 0.13, p = 0.714, partial η2 = 0.000], was found to influence the cognitive reserve. Nevertheless, a significant interaction effect was detected [age group × sex: F(4, 633) = 2.51, p = 0.041, partial η2 = 0.016]. Accordingly, a similar trend between men and women in the age groups from 18–29 to 40–49 was found. However, in the other two age groups (i.e., 50–59 and > 60) significant sex-dependent differences were observed, in particular in men the CRI score reached a plateau at age group 50–59 staying stable in the successive > 60 age group, while it was higher in women than men in the age group 50–59, but lower in the age group > 60. Coherently, Tukey’s HSD post hoc comparisons revealed significant differences between age groups 18–29 and 40–49, 50–59, and > 60, respectively, both in women and men (p values from p < 0.05 to p < 0.0001; Cohen’s d = 1.01 for men 18–29 vs > 60; Cohen’s d = 0.64 for women 18–29 vs > 60) (Fig. 3C for single statistical significances).

Finally, regarding the level of depression, we found a significant effect of the sex [sex: F(1, 633) = 10.88, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.017], but not of age [age group: F(4, 633) = 1.39, p = 0.236, partial η2 = 0.009], and nor of their interaction [age group × sex: F(4, 633) = 2.11, p = 0.078, partial η2 = 0.013]. Moreover, Bonferroni's pairwise comparisons showed that while in the age groups 18–29 to 50–59 men and women exhibited very similar levels of depression, a significant sex-dependent difference was observed for the age group > 60 years (p < 0.002, Cohen’s d = 0.69) (Fig. 3D for single statistical significances).

Relation between odor identification and other variables

To evaluate the associations between OI and each other factor (age, MoCA, CRI, and BDI level), bivariate correlations were carried out (Table 4). Significant negative correlations were found between OI with age (r = − 0.33, p < 0.0001), while significant positive correlations were found with MoCA (r = 0.12, p = 0.003) and CRI (r = 0.21, p < 0.0001) scores. Conversely, no significant correlation between OI and the BDI score was observed, although the p value was very close to the level of significance (p = 0.064) (Table 4).

Table 4 Pearson’s correlations

Multivariate linear regression analysis

Finally, to investigate the contribution of sex, age, BDI, MoCA and CRI level on the OI score an exploratory stepwise multivariate linear regression analysis was performed. The OI was considered as the dependent variable, while sex, age, MoCA, CRI, and BDI scores were used as predictors. In model 1 a significant contribution of age emerged (F(1, 631) = 74.89, p < 0.0001) and the model explained around 10% of variance (R2 = 0.106). Instead, in model 2, a significant effect was observed for age and CRI (F(2, 630) = 76.63, p < 0.0001) with an explanation of about 20% of variance (R2 = 0.196). Finally, in model 3 a significant contribution was observed for sex, age and CRI (F(3, 629) = 53.22, p < 0.0001). Model 3 explained about 20% of variance (R2 = 0.202) (Table 5).

Table 5 Multiple linear regression analyses

Discussion

Compared to the few previous studies [38,39,40], the present results provide new data on olfactory function in different Italian regions. Our results clearly showed significant differences between Italian regions in odor identification scores. OI scores were higher in Sardinia compared to Lazio, Campania, Emilia-Romagna, and Tuscany. The first explanation of differences in OI scores may be associated with environmental factors, pollution, agricultural and cooking practices. Moreover, olfactory function may be better trained in subjects living in natural environmental conditions. OI abilities are related to various conditions including environmental factors, previous experiences, cultural practices, or dietary behavior [11, 49, 50]. Among environmental factors, the exposure to chemical toxins and air pollution may damage the olfactory system as reported in previous studies [51,52,53]. In addition, temperature, humidity, altitude, and air pollution may play a role in olfactory function as indicated in a previous study [50]. The differences in odor identification abilities between Italian regions may be also associated to socioeconomic status, alcohol consumption, education level, and cognitive abilities.

The pleasantness of an odor is related to past experiences and memories [54] with familiar odors being more easily identified compared to unfamiliar ones [31, 55, 56].

Moreover, our results confirmed that OI was correlated to age, sex, and cognitive reserve index (CRI). In line to many previous studies [4, 5, 48, 57] we found a significant reduction in olfactory performance in relation to age increasing. Several theories were proposed for this age-related decrease in olfactory function, which may be related to a reduced number of olfactory receptor neurons, along with a decreased number of fibers in the olfactory bulb [28, 58], changes at a cortical level, changes in the number and in the width of the holes on the cribriform plate, or changes in the mucus composition [57]. Smell sensory loss linked to aging could impair overall health, autonomy, and quality of life as well immunity and appetite contributing also to the development of the so-called anorexia of aging [59, 60]. However, it is important to consider that an olfactory deficit in older individuals is not an inevitable fate [42]. Indeed, older people could show normal olfactory function, so the phenomenon of an age-related impairment should be better investigated, being possibly linked also to a neurodegenerative process, or side effects of drugs [61, 62].

Concerning sex differences in olfactory performance, according to many previous studies [13, 16, 43, 63], our data showed that women exhibited a better odor identification ability than men. Possible explanations for sex differences in olfactory performance were discussed in relation to the female endocrine system and estrogen effects in odor perception [14, 16]. Interestingly, no major sex-related differences were reported in the intranasal volume [64] or in the degree of expression of olfactory receptors [65].

The present results showed a positive association between olfactory function and cognitive reserve index. Previous studies [66, 67] indicated correlations between cognitive abilities and OI. In particular, Yahiaoui-Doktor and colleagues [67] showed that higher olfactory scores were associated with better verbal abilities and semantic memory. Larsson et al. [66] reported an association between age, sex, cognitive speed, and verbal abilities versus odor identification [30]. Indeed, OI task involves high-order cognitive functions and during the olfactory identification process, detection, discrimination, recognition and retrieval of an odor name are requested [30, 68]. Hence, olfactory identification score could be used as a potential early biomarker of mild cognitive impairment in clinical assessments, considering also that it is easy and quick to use without loss of time [47, 69].

The main limitation of the study is the selective use of OI, which represents only a segment of olfactory function. To obtain a more complete evaluation of olfactory performance future studies should investigate also other olfactory domains, such as odor threshold and odor discrimination.

Conclusion

Our study provides data on the prevalence of olfactory dysfunction in a sample of Italian adults. About 27% participants showed hyposmia with an OI score < 12 and differences in OI scores were found among Italian regions. In addition, there was a clear association between OI and age, sex, and CRI with younger women with good CRI exhibiting best scores.