Skip to main content

Entwicklung begrifflichen Wissens: Kernwissenstheorien

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Theorien in der Entwicklungspsychologie

Zusammenfassung

Wie entsteht Wissen? Wie kommen Kinder dazu, Lebewesen von unbelebten Objekten zu unterscheiden, zu zählen und zu rechnen und Begriffe wie Schwerkraft oder Ärger zu verstehen? Und wie kommt die Menschheit dazu, Konzepte wie Evolution oder Atom zu entwickeln? Diese Fragen beschäftigen Psychologie und Philosophie seit deren Anfängen. Seit der Antike spekulieren nativistische Theorien über angeborenes menschliches Wissen, während empiristische Theorien die Metapher der Tabula rasa benützten, um die Position zu untermauern, dass alles Wissen im Laufe des Lebens durch Erfahrung erworben werde. Empirische entwicklungspsychologische Forschung hat seit dem 20. Jahrhundert zur Aufklärung der Herausbildung begrifflichen Wissens in der Ontogenese beigetragen. Insbesondere in den letzten dreißig Jahren sind bei der entwicklungspsychologischen Erforschung des Denkens im Säuglingsalter entscheidende Fortschritte gemacht worden, die zur grundlegenden Revision unserer Vorstellungen über die geistigen Fähigkeiten des menschlichen Säuglings geführt haben. Während William James gegen Ende des 19.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Literatur

  • Apperly, I. A. & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116, 953–970.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aschersleben, G., Hofer, T. & Jovanovic, B. (2008). The link between infant attention to goal-directed action and later theory of mind abilities. Developmental Science, 11, 862–868.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3.5 and 4.5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 655–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R. (1991). Reasoning about the height and location of a hidden object in 4.5- and 6.5-month-old infants. Cognition, 38(1), 13–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M. & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 110–118.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2005). Unwilling versus unable: Infants’ understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology, 41, 328–337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bijeljac-Babic, R., Bertoncini, J. & Mehler, J. (1991). How do four-day-old infants categorize multisyllabic utterances? Developmental Psychology, 29, 711–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandone, A. C. & Wellman, H. M. (2009). You can’t always get what you want. Infants understand failed goaldirected actions. Psychological Science, 20, 85–91.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., Olver, R. R. & Greenfield, P. M. (1966). Studies in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112, 337–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. Hirshfeld & S. Gelman (Hrsg.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (S. 169–200). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V. (1983). Meanings and concepts. In J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman (Hrsg.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3, 4th ed.) (S. 787–840). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clifton, R. K., Rochat, P., Litovsky, R. Y. & Perris, E. E. (1991). Object representation guides infants’ reaching in the dark. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17(2), 323–329.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense. New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D. & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness. A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falck-Ytter, T., Gredebäck, G. & von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 878–879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson, L. & Carey, S. (2003). Tracking individuals via object files: evidence from infants’ manual search. Developmental Science, 6(5), 568–584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson, L. & Carey, S. (2005). On the limits of infants’ quantification of small object arrays. Cognition, 97(3), 295–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson, L., Carey, S. & Hauser, M. D. (2002). The representations underlying infants’ choice of more: Object files versus analog magnitudes. Psychological Science, 13, 150–156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson, L., Carey, S. & Spelke, E. S. (2002). Infants’ discrimination of number vs. continuous extent. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 33–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S. & Spelke, E. S. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 307–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). Modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R. & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The child’s understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., Bekkering, H. & Kiraly, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G. & Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naïve theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G. & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., Glymour, C., Sobel, D. M., Schulz, L. E., Kushnir, T. & Danks, D. (2004). A theory of causal learning in children: Causal maps and Bayes nets. Psychological Review, 111(1), 3–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin, J. K., Hallinan, E. V. & Woodward, A. L. (2008). Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others’ goals. Developmental Science, 11, 487–494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Carey, S. & Hauser, L. B. (2000). Spontaneous number representation in semi-free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Proceedings of Royal Society, London, 267, 829–833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hespos, S. & Baillargeon, R. (2001). Reasoning about containment events in very young infants. Cognition, 78, 207–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hood, B., Carey, S. & Prasada, S. (2000). Predicting the outcomes of physical events: Two-year-olds fail to reveal knowledge of solidity and support. Child Development, 71(6), 1540–1554.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H. & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1994). The birth and nurturance of concepts by domains: The origins of concepts of living things. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Hrsg.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (S. 234–254). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kellman, P. J. & Spelke, E. S. (1983). Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 15(4), 483–524.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, A. M., Teglas, E. & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ belief in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 1830–1834.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luo, Y. & Baillargeon, R. (2007). Do 12.5-month-olds consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions? Cognition, 105, 489–512.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Luo, Y. & Johnson, S. C. (2009). Recognizing the role of perception in action at 6 months. Developmental Science, 12, 142–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-monthold-children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. & Borton, R. W. (1979). Intermodal matching by human neonates. Nature, 282, 403–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702–709.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Onishi, K. & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255–258.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., van Wijngaard, C., Vrins, S., van Rooij, I. & Bekkering, H. (2011). The role of frequency information and teleological reasoning in infants’ and adults’ action prediction. Developmental Psychology, 47, 976–983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. (2009). Who took the cog out of cognitive science? Mentalism in an era of anti-cognitivism. In P. A. Frensch & R. Schwarzer et al (Hrsg.), Cognition and neuropsychology: International perspectives on psychological science (Vol. 1) (S. 241–261). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. & Ruffman, T. (2005). Infants’ insight into the mind: How deep? Science, 308, 214–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, A. T., Wellman, H. M. & Spelke, E. S. (2002). Infants’ ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action. Cognition, 85(1), 53–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The child’s conception of number. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Poulin-Dubois, D., Brooker, I. & Chow, V. (2009). The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy. In P. J. Bauer (Hrsg.), Advances in child development and behaviour (Vol. 37) (S. 55–104). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, R. M. & Baillargeon, R. (2009). Which penguin is this? Attributing false beliefs about object identity at 18 months. Child Development, 80, 1172–1196.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shutts, K., Kenn, R. & Spelke, E. (2006). Object boundaries influence toddlers’ performance in a search task. Developmental Science, 9, 97–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sodian, B. (2011). Theory of Mind in infancy. Child Development Perspectives. 5 (1), 39–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sodian, B. (2012). Denken. In W. Schneider & U. Lindenberger (Hrsg.), Entwicklungspsychologie (7. Aufl.) (S. 385–412). Weinheim: Beltz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sodian, B. & Thoermer, C. (2004). Infants’ understanding of looking, pointing, and reaching as cues to intentional action. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 289–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sodian, B. & Thoermer, C. (2006). Theory of Mind. In W. Schneider & B. Sodian (Hrsg.). Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Serie II: Entwicklungspsychologie, Bd. 2: Kognitive Entwicklung (S. 495–608). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sodian, B. & Thoermer, C. (2008). Precursors to a theory of mind in infancy: Perspectives for research on autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 27–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sodian, B., Thoermer, C. & Metz, U. (2007). Now I see it but you don’t: 14-month-olds can represent another person’s visual perspective. Developmental Science, 10, 199–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96, B1–B11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S. (1994). Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. Cognition, 50, 431–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S., Breilinger, K., Macomber, J. & Jacobsen, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605–632.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S. & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10(1), 89–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Starkey, P. & Cooper, R. (1980). Perception of numbers by human infants. Science, 210, 1033–1035.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thoermer, C., Sodian, B., Vuori, M., Perst, H. & Kristen, S. (2012). Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 172–187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Träuble, B., Marinovic, V. & Pauen, S. (2010). Early theory of mind competencies – Do infants understand others’ beliefs? Infancy, 15, 434–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valenza, E., Gava, L., Leo, I. & Simeon, F. (2006). Perceptual completion in newborn human infants. Child Development, 77(6), 1810–1821.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M. (2011). Developing a theory of mind. In U. Goswami (Hrsg.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (2nd ed.) (S. 258–284). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D. & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis for theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M. & Gelman, S. A. (1998). Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. In W. Damon (Hrsg.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 2: Cognition, perception, and language (S. 523–573). Hoboken, NJ, US: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Lopez-Duran, S., LaBounty, J. & Hamilton, B. (2008). Infant attention of intentional action predicts preschool social cognition. Developmental Psychology, 44, 618–623.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Phillips, A. T., Dunphy-Lelii, S. & LaLonde, N. (2004). Infant social attention predicts preschool social cognition. Developmental Science, 7, 283–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, A. L. & Guajardo, J. J. (2002). Infants’ understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action. Cognitive Development, 17, 1061–1084.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, K. (1990). Children’s understanding of counting. Cognition, 36, 155–193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, K. (1992a). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358, 749–750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, K. (1992b). Children’s acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 220–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, K. (1996). Infants’ individuation and enumeration of actions. Psychological Science, 7, 164–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F. (2011). Rational constructivism, statistical inference and core cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 151–152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F. & Griffiths, T. L. (2011). Probabilistic models of cognitive development: Towards a rational constructivist approach to the study of learning and development. Cognition, 120, 299–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F. & Spelke, E. S. (2000). Large number discrimination in 6-month old infants. Cognition, 74, B1–B11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F. & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Sensitivity to sampling in Bayesian word learning. Developmental Science, 10(3), 288–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Beate Sodian .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sodian, B. (2014). Entwicklung begrifflichen Wissens: Kernwissenstheorien. In: Ahnert, L. (eds) Theorien in der Entwicklungspsychologie. Springer VS, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-34805-1_5

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-34805-1_5

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer VS, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-34804-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-34805-1

  • eBook Packages: Humanities, Social Science (German Language)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics