Skip to main content

Tax Compliance: Research Methods and Decision Processes

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Psychological Perspectives on Financial Decision Making

Abstract

Traditionally, research focuses on individual taxpayers that—when faced with a decision under uncertainty—are assumed to maximize their profits through rational decision processes. However, economic psychology and behavioral economics reveal several anomalies where the observed effects are opposite to the theoretical predictions. Moreover, psychological research provides evidence for the importance of factors such as the understanding of the tax law, attitudes toward taxes and tax morale, personal and social norms, and perception of distributive and procedural justice. In this paper, we provide a review of the research on tax compliance decisions. We address traditional approaches to study compliance decisions and anomalies as well as the psychological determinants of compliance. Since different research methods reveal different results, we describe the arsenal of research methods and their strengths and weaknesses. We pay specific attention to results from process tracing approaches in tax compliance research. We conclude with practical implications for policymakers and researchers in the field.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adams, C., & Webley, P. (2001). Small business owners’ attitudes on VAT compliance in the UK. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22(2), 195–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allingham, M. G., & Sandmo, A. (1972). Income tax evasion: A theoretical analysis. Journal of Public Economics, 1(3–4), 323–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J. (2012). Measuring, explaining, and controlling tax evasion: Lessons from theory, experiments, and field studies. International Tax and Public Finance, 19(1), 54–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Bloomquist, K. M., & McKee, M. (2015). On the external validity of laboratory tax compliance experiments. Economic Inquiry, 53(2), 1170–1186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Bloomquist, K. M., & McKee, M. (2017). When you know your neighbour pays taxes: Information, peer effects and tax compliance. Fiscal Studies, 38(4), 587–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Cherry, T., Jones, M., & McKee, M. (2010). Taxpayer information assistance services and tax compliance behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(4), 577–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Jackson, B. R., & McKee, M. (2009). Getting the word out: Enforcement information dissemination and compliance behavior. Journal of Public Economics, 93(3), 392–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Kirchler, E., & Muehlbacher, S. (2012). Combining psychology and economics in the analysis of compliance: From enforcement to cooperation. Economic Analysis and Policy, 42(2), 133–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., McClelland, G. H., & Schulze, W. D. (1992). Why do people pay taxes? Journal of Public Economics, 48(1), 21–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., & Torgler, B. (2006). Culture differences and tax morale in the United States and in Europe. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(2), 224–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., & Torgler, B. (2011). Do ethics matter? Tax compliance and morality. Journal of Business Ethics, 101(4), 635–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni, J., Erard, B., & Feinstein, J. (1998). Tax compliance. Journal of Economic Literature, 36(2), 818–860.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbex, M., Carre, J. M., Geniole, S. N., & Mattos, E. (2018). Testosterone, personality traits and tax evasion (No. 18–01). Retrieved from University of Windsor, Department of Economics website: http://web2.uwindsor.ca/economics/RePEc/wis/pdf/1801.pdf

  • Arkes, H., & Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35(1), 124–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arkes, H., Joyner, C. A., Pezzo, M. V., Nash, J. G., Karen, S.-J., & Stone, E. (1994). The psychology of windfall gains. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 59(3), 331–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, N. J. S., Yechiam, E., & Ben-Eliezer, D. (2018). The consistency of visual attention to losses and loss sensitivity across valuation and choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(12), 1791–1809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balconi, M., Crivelli, D., Castiglioni, C., & Lozza, E. (2019). Social frame and tax compliance modulate electrophysiological and autonomic responses following tax-related decisions. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 4713.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Batrancea, L., Nichita, A., Olsen, J., Kogler, C., Kirchler, E., Hoelzl, E., … Zukauskas, S. (2019). Trust and power as determinants of tax compliance across 44 nations. Journal of Economic Psychology, 74, 102191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S. (1968). Crime and punishment: An economic approach. Journal of Political Economy, 76(2), 169–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, C. G., & Buchanan, W. (1966). Reliable and unreliable respondents: Party registration and prestige pressure. Western Political Science Association, 19(1), 37–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, C. M., Wolford, G. L., & Miller, M. B. (2009). The principled control of false positives in neuroimaging. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(4), 417–422.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bergman, M., & Nevarez, A. (2006). Do audits enhance compliance? An empirical assessment of VAT enforcement. National Tax Journal, 59(4), 817–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernasconi, M., & Zanardi, A. (2004). Tax evasion, tax rates, and reference dependence. FinanzArchiv/Public Finance Analysis, 60(3), 422–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berns, G. S., & Moore, S. E. (2012). A neural predictor of cultural popularity. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(1), 154–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bettman, J. R., Johnson, E. J., & Payne, J. W. (1990). A componential analysis of cognitive effort in choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 45(1), 111–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumenthal, M., Christian, C., Slemrod, J., & Smith, M. G. (2001). Do normative appeals affect tax compliance? Evidence from a controlled experiment in Minnesota. National Tax Journal, 54(1), 125–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bobek, D. D., Hageman, A. M., & Kelliher, C. F. (2013). Analyzing the role of social norms in tax compliance behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 115(3), 451–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boylan, S. J., & Sprinkle, G. B. (2001). Experimental evidence on the relation between tax rates and compliance: The effect of earned vs. endowed income. The Journal of the American Taxation Association, 23(1), 75–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braithwaite, V. (2003). Dancing with tax authorities: Motivational postures and non-compliant actions. In Taxing democracy: Understanding tax avoidance and evasion (pp. 15–39). Oxon, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. B., & Crano, W. D. (2000). Research design and issues of validity. In Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bristow, A. L., Wardman, M., Zanni, A. M., & Chintakayala, P. K. (2010). Public acceptability of personal carbon trading and carbon tax. Ecological Economics, 69(9), 1824–1837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2005). Neuroeconomics: How neuroscience can inform economics. Journal of Economic Literature, 43(1), 9–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C. F., Cohen, J., Fehr, E., Glimcher, P., & Laibson, D. (2015). Neuroeconomics. In J. Kagel & R. Roth (Eds.), Handbook of experimental economics (Vol. 2). Princeton, NJ/Oxford, UK: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, M. C., Anderson, J. R., & Sohn, M. H. (2001). What can a mouse cursor tell us more?: Correlation of eye/mouse movements on web browsing. CHI’01 extended abstracts on human factors in computing systems, pp. 281–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, T. L., Frykblom, P., & Shogren, J. F. (2002). Hardnose the dictator. American Economic Review, 92(4), 1218–1221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colbert, G., Murray, D., & Nieschwietz, R. (2009). The use of expected value in pricing judgments. Journal of Risk Research, 12(2), 199–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, S. (1996). The Minnesota income tax compliance experiment: State tax results. Retrieved from https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/4827/1/MPRA_paper_4827.pdf

  • Coleman, S. (2007). The Minnesota income tax compliance experiment: Replication of the social norms experiment (MPRA paper no. 5820). Retrieved from http://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/4827/

  • Collins, J., & Plumlee, R. (1991). The taxpayer’s labor and reporting decision: The effect of audit schemes. The Accounting Review, 66(3), 559–576.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coricelli, G., Joffily, M., Montmarquette, C., & Villeval, M. C. (2010). Cheating, emotions, and rationality: An experiment on tax evasion. Experimental Economics, 13(2), 226–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coricelli, G., Rusconi, E., & Villeval, M. C. (2014). Tax evasion and emotions: An empirical test of re-integrative shaming theory. Journal of Economic Psychology, 40, 49–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, V. P., & Meng, J. (2011). New York City cab drivers’ labor supply revisited: Reference-dependent preferences with rational-expectations targets for hours and income. American Economic Review, 101(5), 1912–1932.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuccia, A. D., & Carnes, G. A. (2001). A closer look at the relation between tax complexity and tax equity perceptions. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22(2), 113–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawes, R. M. (1980). Social dilemmas. Annual Review of Psychology, 31(1), 169–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Widt, D., & Oats, L. (2017). Risk assessment in a co-operative compliance context: A Dutch- UK comparison. British Tax Review, 2, 230–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dulleck, U., Fooken, J., Newton, C., Ristl, A., Schaffner, M., & Torgler, B. (2016). Tax compliance and psychic costs: Behavioral experimental evidence using a physiological marker. Journal of Public Economics, 134, 9–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durham, Y., Manly, T. S., & Ritsema, C. (2014). The effects of income source, context, and income level on tax compliance decisions in a dynamic experiment. Journal of Economic Psychology, 40, 220–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elffers, H., & Hessing, D. J. (1997). Influencing the prospects of tax evasion. Journal of Economic Psychology, 18(2–3), 289–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elffers, H., Robben, H. S. J., & Hessing, D. J. (1992). On measuring tax evasion. Journal of Economic Psychology, 13(4), 545–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elffers, H., Weigel, R. H., & Hessing, D. J. (1987). The consequences of different strategies for measuring tax evasion behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 8(3), 311–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enachescu, J., Zieser, M., Hofmann, E., & Kirchler, E. (2019). Horizontal monitoring in Austria: Subjective representations by tax officials and company employees. Business Research, 12(1), 75–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engström, P., Nordblom, K., Ohlsson, H., & Persson, A. (2015). Tax compliance and loss aversion. American Economic Journal: Economic Policy, 7(4), 132–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, K., & Fallan, L. (1996). Tax knowledge and attitudes towards taxation; A report on a quasi-experiment. Journal of Economic Psychology, 17(3), 387–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fallan, L. (1999). Gender, exposure to tax knowledge, and attitudes towards taxation; an experimental approach. Journal of Business Ethics, 18(2), 173–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrington, D. P. (1973). Self-reports of deviant behavior: Predictive and stable. The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 64, 99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feld, L. P., & Frey, B. S. (2007). Tax compliance as the result of a psychological tax contract: The role of incentives and responsive regulation. Law & Policy, 29(1), 102–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feld, L. P., Frey, B. S., & Torgler, B. (2006). Rewarding honest taxpayers. In Managing and maintaining compliance (pp. 45–61). The Hague, the Netherlands: BJu Legal Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischbacher, U., Gachter, S., & Fehr, E. (2001). Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Economics Letters, 8, 397–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, C. M., Wartick, M., & Mark, M. (1992). Detection probability and taxpayer compliance: A review of the literature. Journal of Accounting Literature, 11, 1–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fjeldstad, H., & Semboja, J. (2001). Why people pay taxes: The case of the development levy in Tanzania. World Development, 29(12), 1059–2074.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frame, M. E. (2019). EEG and ERPS as neural process tracing methodologies in decision making research. In M. Schulte-Mecklenbeck, A. Kuehberger, & J. G. Johnson (Eds.), A handbook of process tracing methods (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franco-Watkins, A. M., & Johnson, J. G. (2011). Applying the decision moving window to risky choice: Comparison of eye-tracking and mousetracing methods. Judgment and Decision making, 6(8), 740–749.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2010). MouseTracker: Software for studying real-time mental processing using a computer mouse-tracking method. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 226–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S. (1997). Not Just for the money. London, UK: Edward Elgar Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Torgler, B. (2007). Tax morale and conditional cooperation. Journal of Comparative Economics, 35(1), 136–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedland, N., Maital, S., & Rutenberg, A. (1978). A simulation study of income tax evasion. Journal of Public Economics, 10, 107–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gal, D., & Rucker, D. D. (2018). The loss of loss aversion: Will it loom larger than its gain? Journal of Consumer Psychology, 28(3), 497–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gangl, K., Hofmann, E., & Kirchler, E. (2015). Tax authorities’ interaction with taxpayers: A conception of compliance in social dilemmas by power and trust. New Ideas in Psychology, 37, 13–23.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gangl, K., Pfabigan, D. M., Lamm, C., Kirchler, E., & Hofmann, E. (2017). Coercive and legitimate authority impact tax honesty: Evidence from behavioral and ERP experiments. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(7), 1108–1117.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Glöckner, A., & Herbold, A.-K. (2011). An eye-tracking study on information processing in risky decisions: Evidence for compensatory strategies based on automatic processes. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 24(1), 71–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goslinga, S., Siglé, M., & Veldhuizen, R. (2019). Cooperative compliance, tax control frameworks and perceived certainty about the tax position in large organisations. Journal of Tax Administration, 5(1), 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grundmann, S., & Graf Lambsdorff, J. (2017). How income and tax rates provoke cheating—An experimental investigation of tax morale. Journal of Economic Psychology, 63, 27–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haavio, M., & Kotakorpi, K. (2011). The political economy of sin taxes. European Economic Review, 55(4), 575–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hallsworth, M. (2014). The use of field experiments to increase tax compliance. Oxford Review of Economic Policy, 30(4), 658–679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harbaugh, W. T., Mayr, U., & Burghart, D. R. (2007). Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science, 316(5831), 1622–1625.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartner, M., Rechberger, S., Kirchler, E., & Schabmann, A. (2008). Procedural fairness and tax compliance. Economic Analysis and Policy, 38(1), 137–152.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, C., & Soll, J. B. (1996). Mental budgeting and consumer decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 23(1), 40–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heffetz, O., & List, J. A. (2014). Is the endowment effect an expectations effect? Journal of the European Economic Association, 12(5), 1396–1422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hochman, G., Glöckner, A., Fiedler, S., & Ayal, S. (2016). “I can see it in your eyes”: Biased processing and increased arousal in dishonest responses: Biased processing and arousal in dishonesty. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29(2–3), 322–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inglehart, R., Haerpfer, C., Moreno, A., Welzel, C., Kizilova, K., Diez-Medrano, J., …, Puranen, B. (2014). World values survey: Round one—Country-pooled data file version. Retrieved from JD Systems Institute website: http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/WVSDocumentationWV1.jsp

  • Internal Revenue Service. (2016). Data book, 2015 (Publication 55B). Retrieved from https://www.irs.gov/pub/irs-soi/15databk.pdf

  • Jackson, B. R., & Milliron, V. C. (1986). Tax compliance research: Findings, problems, and prospects. Journal of Accounting Literature, 5(1), 125–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, R. J. K., & Karn, K. S. (2003). Commentary on section 4—Eye tracking in human-computer interaction and usability research: Ready to deliver the promises. In J. Hyönä, R. Radach, & H. Deubel (Eds.), The mind’s eye (pp. 573–605). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jimenez, P., & Iyer, G. S. (2016). Tax compliance in a social setting: The influence of social norms, trust in government, and perceived fairness on taxpayer compliance. Advances in Accounting, 34, 17–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Well-being: Foundations of hedonic psychology. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1986). Rational choice and the framing of decisions. Journal of Business, 59(4), 251–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamleitner, B., & Hoelzl, E. (2009). Cost-benefit associations and financial behavior. Applied Psychology, 58(3), 435–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kastlunger, B., Kirchler, E., Mittone, L., & Pitters, J. (2009). Sequences of audits, tax compliance, and taxpaying strategies. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30(3), 405–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kastlunger, B., Lozza, E., Kirchler, E., & Schabmann, A. (2013). Powerful authorities and trusting citizens: The slippery slope framework and tax compliance in Italy. Journal of Economic Psychology, 34, 36–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kieslich, P. J., Henninger, F., Wulff, D. U., Haslbeck, J. M. B., & Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M. (2019). Mouse-tracking: A practical guide to implementation and analysis. In M. Schulte-Mecklenbeck, A. Kühberger, & J. G. Johnson (Eds.), A Handbook of Process Tracing Methods (pp. 111–130). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kieslich, P. J., & Hilbig, B. E. (2014). Cognitive conflict in social dilemmas: An analysis of response dynamics. Judgment and Decision making, 9(6), 13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, B., Seligman, D., & Kable, J. W. (2012). Preference reversals in decision making under risk are accompanied by changes in attention to different attributes. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, 109.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E. (1997). The burden of new taxes: Acceptance of taxes as a function of affectedness and egoistic versus altruistic orientation. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 26(4), 421–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E. (1998). Differential representations of taxes: Analysis of free associations and judgments of five employment groups. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 27(1), 117–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E. (2007). The economic psychology of tax behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E., & Hoelzl, E. (2017). Economic psychology: An introduction. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E., Hoelzl, E., & Wahl, I. (2008). Enforced versus voluntary tax compliance: The “slippery slope” framework. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(2), 210–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E., Kogler, C., & Muehlbacher, S. (2014). Cooperative tax compliance: From deterrence to deference. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(2), 87–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E., & Maciejovsky, B. (2001). Tax compliance within the context of gain and loss situations, expected and current asset position, and profession. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22, 173–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E., & Muehlbacher, S. (2010). Das Slippery Slope Framework des Steuerverhaltens – Zum Einfluss von Macht und Vertrauen auf erzwungene und freiwillige Kooperation. In E. H. Witte & T. Gollan (Eds.), Sozialpsychologie und Ökonomie (pp. 173–182). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E., Muehlbacher, S., Hoelzl, E., & Webley, P. (2009). Effort and aspirations in tax evasion: Experimental evidence. Applied Psychology. An International Review, 58(3), 488–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E., Muehlbacher, S., Kastlunger, B., & Wahl, I. (2010). Why pay taxes? A review of tax compliance decisions. In J. Alm, J. Martinez-Vazques, & B. Torgler (Eds.), Developing alternative frameworks for explaining tax compliance (pp. 15–31). London, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E., & Muhlbacher, S. (2007). Kontrollen und Sanktionen im Steuerstrafrecht aus der Sicht der Rechtspsychologie. In R. Leitner (Ed.), Finanzstrafrecht (pp. 9–33). Vienna, Austria: Linde Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kogler, C., Muehlbacher, S., & Kirchler, E. (2015). Testing the “slippery slope framework” among self-employed taxpayers. Economics of Governance, 16(2), 125–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kogler, C., Olsen, J., Mueller, M., & Kirchler, E. (2019). Testing the Allingham and Sandmo model of income tax evasion: A process tracing approach. Manuscript in preparation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kogler, C., Olsen, J., Müller, M., & Kirchler, E. (2020, May 18). Information processing in tax decisions: A MouselabWEB study on the Allingham and Sandmo model of income tax evasion. Retrieved from https://www.psyarxiv.com/bpe8j

  • Koop, G. J., & Johnson, J. G. (2011). Response dynamics: A new window on the decision process. Judgment and Decision making, 6(8), 750–758.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraut, R., Olson, J., Banaji, M., Bruckman, A., Cohen, J., & Couper, M. (2004). Psychological research online: Report of board of scientific affairs’ advisory group on the conduct of research on the internet. American Psychologist, 59(2), 105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lago-Peñas, I., & Lago-Peñas, S. (2010). The determinants of tax morale in comparative perspective: Evidence from European countries. European Journal of Political Economy, 26(4), 441–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lejarraga, T., Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., Pachur, T., & Hertwig, R. (2019). The attention–aversion gap: How allocation of attention relates to loss aversion. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40(5), 457–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levitt, S. D., & List, J. A. (2007). What do laboratory experiments measuring social preferences reveal about the real world? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(2), 153–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, S. (2003). The role of expected value illustrated in decision-making under risk: Single-play vs multiple-play. Journal of Risk Research, 6(2), 113–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenstein, S., & Slovic, P. (1971). Reversals of preference between bids and choices in gambling decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89(1), 46–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenstein, S., Slovic, P., & Zink, D. (1969). Effect of instruction in expected value on optimality of gambling decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 79(2), 236–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G., & Issacharoff, S. (1994). Source dependence in the valuation of objects. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 7(3), 157–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maciejovsky, B., Kirchler, E., & Schwarzenberger, H. (2007). Misperception of chance and loss repair: On the dynamics of tax compliance. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(6), 678–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marzilli Ericson, K. M., & Fuster, A. (2011). Expectations as endowments: Evidence on reference-dependent preferences from exchange and valuation experiments. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 126(4), 1879–1907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mascagni, G. (2018). From the lab to the field: A review of tax experiments. Journal of Economic Surveys, 32(2), 273–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 20(3), 709–734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCaffery, E. J., & Baron, J. (2006). Thinking about tax. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 12(1), 106–135. Retrieved from https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=567767

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKerchar, M. (2001). Why do taxpayers comply—Past lessons and future directions in developing a model of compliance behaviour. Australian Tax Forum, 16, 99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mittone, L. (2006). Dynamic behaviour in tax evasion: An experimental approach. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 35(5), 813–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moscovici, S. (1961). La psychanalyse, son image et son public. Paris, France: Presses universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muehlbacher, S., Hartl, B., & Kirchler, E. (2017). Mental accounting and tax compliance: Experimental evidence for the effect of mental segregation of tax due and revenue on compliance. Public Finance Review, 45(1), 118–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muehlbacher, S., & Kirchler, E. (2009). Origin of endowments in public good games: The impact of effort on contributions. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 2(1), 59–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muehlbacher, S., & Kirchler, E. (2013). Mental accounting of self-employed taxpayers: On the mental segregation of the net income and the tax due. SSRN Electronic Journal, 69(4), 412–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muehlbacher, S., & Kirchler, E. (2016). Experiments in tax compliance research. Die Betriebswirtschaft, 76(1), 7–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muehlbacher, S., & Zieser, M. (2018). Die Psychologie des Steuerzahlens. In Die Psychologie des Steuerzahlens. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nagin, D. S. (2013). Deterrence in the twenty-first century: A review of the evidence. Crime and Justice, 42(1), 199–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OECD. (2013). Co-operative compliance: A framework. Retrieved October 7, 2019, from https://www.oecd.org/publications/co-operative-compliance-a-framework-9789264200852-en.htm

  • Oishi, S., Schimmack, U., & Diener, E. (2012). Progressive taxation and the subjective well-being of nations. Psychological Science, 23(1), 86–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olsen, J., Kasper, M., Kogler, C., Muehlbacher, S., & Kirchler, E. (2019). Mental accounting of income tax and value added tax among self-employed business owners. Journal of Economic Psychology, 70, 125–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orquin, J. L., & Mueller Loose, S. (2013). Attention and choice: A review on eye movements in decision making. Acta Psychologica, 144(1), 190–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pachur, T., Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., Murphy, R. O., & Hertwig, R. (2018). Prospect theory reflects selective allocation of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(2), 147–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W. (1976). Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: An information search and protocol analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16(2), 366–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poole, A., Ball, L. J., & Phillips, P. (2005). In search of salience: A response-time and eye-movement analysis of bookmark recognition. In S. Fincher, P. Markopoulos, D. Moore, & R. Ruddle (Eds.), People and computers XVIII—Design for life (pp. 363–378). London, UK: Springer London.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Prelec, D., & Loewenstein, G. (1998). The red and the black: Mental accounting of savings and debt. Marketing Science, 17(1), 4–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ranyard, R., Hinkley, L., Williamson, J., & McHugh, S. (2006). The role of mental accounting in consumer credit decision processes. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(4), 571–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rees-Jones, A. (2018). Quantifying loss-averse tax manipulation. Review of Economic Studies, 85(2), 1251–1278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robben, H. S. J., Webley, P., Weigel, R. H., Wärneryd, K.-E., Kinsey, K. A., Hessing, D. J., … Scholz, J. T. (1990). Decision frame and opportunity as determinants of tax cheating: An international experimental study. Journal of Economic Psychology, 11(3), 341–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roe, B. E., & Just, D. R. (2009). Internal and external validity in economics research: Tradeoffs between experiments, field experiments, natural experiments, and field data. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, 91(5), 1266–1271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothstein, B. (2000). Trust, social dilemmas and collective memories. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 12(4), 477–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruffle, B. J. (1998). More is better, but fair is fair: Tipping in dictator and ultimatum games. Games and Economic Behavior, 23(2), 247–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sakurai, Y., & Braithwaite, V. (2003). Taxpayers’ perceptions of practitioners: Finding one who is effective and does the right thing? Journal of Business Ethics, 46(4), 375–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, New Series, 300(5626), 1755–1758.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schepanski, A., & Shearer, T. (1995). A prospect theory account of the income tax withholding phenomenon. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63(2), 174–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., Kühberger, A., Gagl, B., & Hutzler, F. (2017). Inducing thought processes: Bringing process measures and cognitive processes closer together. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(5), 1001–1013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., Kühberger, A., & Ranyard, R. (2011). The role of process data in the development and testing of process models of judgment and decision making. Judgment and Decision making, 6(8), 733.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., Sohn, M., de Bellis, E., Martin, N., & Hertwig, R. (2013). A lack of appetite for information and computation. Simple heuristics in food choice. Appetite, 71, 242–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston, MA: Houghton, Mifflin, and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shu, L. L., Mazar, N., Gino, F., Ariely, D., & Bazerman, M. H. (2012). Signing at the beginning makes ethics salient and decreases dishonest self-reports in comparison to signing at the end. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(38), 15197–15200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sickmann, J., & Ngan, L. (2016). Eye-tracking in behavioural economics and finance: A literature review (Discussion Papers in Behavioural Sciences and Economics, Vol. 1).

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychological Review, 63(2), 129–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1983). Preference reversals: A broader perspective. The American Economic Review, 73(4), 596–605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spicer, M. W., & Lundstedt, S. B. (1976). Understanding tax evasion. Public Finance = Finances Publiques, 31(2), 295–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spivey, M. J., & Dale, R. (2006). Continuous dynamics in real-time cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(5), 207–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Srinivasan, T. (1973). Tax evasion: A model. Journal of Public Economics, 2(4), 339–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strümpel, B. (1969). The contribution of survey research to public finance. In A. T. Peacock & D. Biehl (Eds.), Quantitative analysis in public finance (pp. 14–32). New York, NY: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Svenson, O. (1979). Process descriptions of decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 23(1), 86–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. (1980). Toward a positive theory of consumer choice. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 1(1), 39–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. (1985). Mental accounting and consumer choice. Marketing Science, 4(3), 199–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. (1999). Mental accounting matters. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12(3), 183–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R., & Johnson, E. J. (1990). Gambling with the house money and trying to break even: The effects of prior outcomes on risky choice. Management Science, 36(6), 643–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth and happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B. (2004). Tax morale in Asian countries. Journal of Asian Economics, 15(2), 237–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B. (2006). The importance of faith: Tax morale and religiosity. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 61(1), 81–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B., & Schneider, F. (2007). What shapes attitudes toward paying taxes? Evidence from multicultural European countries. Social Science Quarterly, 88(2), 443–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B., & Schneider, F. (2009). The impact of tax morale and institutional quality on the shadow economy. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30(2), 228–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), 207–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R. (2006). Why people obey the law. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R., Goff, P. A., & MacCoun, R. J. (2015). The impact of psychological science on policing in the United States: Procedural justice, legitimacy, and effective law enforcement. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 16(3), 75–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Venkatraman, V., Dimoka, A., Pavlou, P. A., Vo, K., Hampton, W., Bollinger, B., … Winer, R. S. (2015). Predicting advertising success beyond traditional measures: New insights from neurophysiological methods and market response modeling. Journal of Marketing Research, 52(4), 436–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venkatraman, V., & Reeck, C. (2019). Decision neuroscience. In M. Schulte-Mecklenbeck, A. Kuehberger, & J. G. Johnson (Eds.), A handbook of process tracing methods (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1947). Theory of games and economic behavior (2nd rev. ed.). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wahl, I., Muehlbacher, S., & Kirchler, E. (2010). The impact of voting on tax payments. Kyklos, 63(1), 144–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M. (2004). An analysis of norm processes in tax compliance. Journal of Economic Psychology, 25(2), 213–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M. (2005). Misperceptions of social norms about tax compliance: From theory to intervention. Journal of Economic Psychology, 26(6), 862–883.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M., & Taylor, N. (2004). An experimental evaluation of tax-reporting schedules: A case of evidence-based tax administration. Journal of Public Economics, 88(12), 2785–2799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willemsen, M. C., & Johnson, E. J. (2011). Visiting the decision factory: Observing cognition with MouselabWEB and other information acquisition methods. In M. Schulte-Mecklenbeck, A. Kuehberger, & R. Ranyard (Eds.), Society for Judgment and Decision Making series. A handbook of process tracing methods for decision research: A critical review and user’s guide (pp. 21–42). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg, M., & Van Dijk, E. (1997). A reverse sunk cost effect in risky decision making: Sometimes we have too much invested to gamble. Journal of Economic Psychology, 18(6), 677–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zmerli, S., & Castillo, J. C. (2015). Income inequality, distributive fairness and political trust in Latin America. Social Science Research, 52, 179–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andre Julian Hartmann .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Hartmann, A.J., Mueller, M., Kirchler, E. (2020). Tax Compliance: Research Methods and Decision Processes. In: Zaleskiewicz, T., Traczyk, J. (eds) Psychological Perspectives on Financial Decision Making. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-45500-2_13

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics