Skip to main content
Log in

Children’s Narrating as a Way of Learning About Other People’s Beliefs in Interaction with Teachers

  • Published:
Early Childhood Education Journal Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

As a genre of talk, narratives represent important building blocks in children’s learning in many fields. The purpose of the study presented in this article is to examine how teachers can encourage children’s learning about people’s beliefs through narrating. Narratives play an important part in children’s learning to understand other people and how they will act according to what they believe, think or know. This study is based on video observations of six children, 3 and 4 years old, and their spontaneous personal narratives told to teachers over a period of 8 months. The narrative analysis revealed that in most of the narratives the teachers were passive listeners or were concerned about the structure of events. The teachers seldom asked questions about the children’s mental state or disagreed with the child in ways that revealed their different beliefs. Suggestions about implications of this study are that early childhood teacher education should focus on talking with children about what they may think or believe concerning narrated events, and also reveal what they think and believe themselves.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The term Kindergarten refers to the Norwegian Kindergarten (barnehage) for children from 1 to 6 years of age.

  2. Ole did not pass the False Belief test (Perner et al. 1987), but Vera did.

References

  • Aukrust, V. G., & Snow, C. E. (1998). Narratives and explanations in Norwegian and American mealtime conversations. Language in Society, 27, 221–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, D. L., Katz, T., & Champion, L. (2003). Young children’s narratives and ideologies in classrooms. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 19, 205–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borg, E., Kristiansen, I., & Backe-Hansen, E. (2008). Kvalitet og innhold i norske barnehager. En kunnskapsoversikt fra NOVANorsk institutt for forskning om oppvekst, velferd og aldring. Oslo: Rapport nr 6/08.

  • Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1992). The narrative construction of reality. In B. Beilin (Ed.), Piaget’s theory: Prospects and possibilities (pp. 229–251). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J., & Feldman, C. (1996). Group narrative as a cultural context of autobiography. In D. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory (pp. 291–318). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D. K., & Caswell, L. (2007). Building support for early literacy in preschool classroom through in-service professional development: Effects of the Literacy Environment Enrichment Program (LEEP). Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22, 243–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gjems, L. (2006). What do children learn through narrating? A study of children’s learning through narrative practices. Oslo: University of Oslo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gulbrandsen, L., Johansson, J., & Nilsen, R. D. (2002). Forskning om barnehager. En kunnskapsstatus: Norges forskningsråd. Område for Kultur og samfunn.

  • Halliday, M. A. K. (1993). Towards a language-based theory of learning. Linguistics and Education, 5, 93–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasan, R. (2002a). Semiotic mediation and mental development in pluralistic societies: Some implications for tomorrow’s schooling. In G. Wells & G. Claxton (Eds.), Learing for life in the 21st century: Socio-cultural perspectives on the future of education (pp. 112–127). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasan, R. (2002b). Ways of meaning, ways of learning: Code as an explanatory concept. British Journal of Sociology of Education, 23(4), 537–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, S. B. (1982). What no bedtime story means: Narrative skills at home and school. Language and society (Vol. II, pp. 49–76). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, E., Torrance, N., & Olson, D. R. (2001). Young children and the say/mean distinction: Verbatim and paraphrase recognition in narrative and nursery rhyme contexts. Child Language, 28, 531–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74(4), 1130–1144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (1991). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, N., Fernandez, M., Dawes, L., Wegerif, R., & Sams, C. (2003). Talk about texts at the computer: Using ICT to develop children’s oral and literate abilities. Reading Literacy and Language, 37(2), 81–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1989). Monologue as representation of real life experience. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 27–73). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development. The emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (2000). Narrative, time and the emergence of the encultured self. Culture and Psychology, 1 & 2, 183–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (2003). Narrative self, myth and memory: Emergence of the cultural self. In R. Fivush & C. Haden (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 3–28). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

  • Nelson, K. (2004). Language pathways into the community of minds. In J. Astington & J. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for the theory of mind (pp. 26–50). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K., & Kessler Shaw, L. (2002). Developing a socially shared symbolic system. In J. Amsel & E. Byrnes (Eds.), Language, literacy and cognitive development (pp. 27–61). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Nelson, K., Plesa, D., Skwerer, S., Goldman, S., Henseler, N., Presler, S., et al. (2003). Entering a community of minds: An experimental approach to “theory of mind”. Human Development 1998, 46, 24–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicolopoulou, A. (1997). Children and narratives: Towards an interpretive and sociocultural approach. In M. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative ability and human development (pp. 179–217). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ninio, A., & Snow, C. E. (1996). Pragmatic development. Boulder: Westview Press Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (2001). Living narrative. Creating lives in everyday storytelling. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-years olds’ difficulty with false beliefs. The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shwe, H. I., & Markman, E. (1997). Young children’s appreciation of the mental impact of their communicative signals. Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 630–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siraj-Blatchford, I., & Manni, L. (2008). Would you like to tidy up now? An analysis of adult questioning in the English Foundation Stage. Early Years, 28(1), 5–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterck, E. H. M., & Begeer, S. (2010). Theory of mind: Specialized capacity or emergent property? European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1978). Tænkning og sprog II. København: A. Diderichsen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about the false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. (2007). Semiotic mediation, dialogue and the construction of knowledge. Human Development, 50, 244–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G., & Araus, R. M. (2006). Dialogues in the classroom. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15(3), 379–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G., & Nassaji, H. (2000). What’s the use of triadic dialogue? An investigation of teacher-student interaction. Applied Linguistic, 21(3), 376–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Liv Gjems.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gjems, L. Children’s Narrating as a Way of Learning About Other People’s Beliefs in Interaction with Teachers. Early Childhood Educ J 38, 271–278 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-010-0413-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-010-0413-1

Keywords

Navigation